Review of General Psychology
American Psychological Association, Inc.
Volume 5(4) December 2001
p 382–405
The Importance of Father Love: History and Contemporary Evidence
[Articles]
Rohner, Ronald P.1,3; Veneziano, Robert A.2
1Center for the Study of Parental Acceptance and Rejection,
University of Connecticut
2Department of Social Work, Western Connecticut State
University.
3Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Ronald P. Rohner, Center for the Study of Parental Acceptance and Rejection,
U-58 Family Studies, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut 06269-2058.
Electronic mail may be sent to Rohner@uconn.edu.
Received Date: October 27, 2000; Revised Date: April 10, 2001; Accepted
Date: April 10, 2001
Outline
· Abstract
· Fathers: The Historically Understudied
· Fatherhood Is a Cultural Construction
· Growing Awareness of the Influence of Father
Love
· Acknowledging the Importance of Fathers and
Father Love
· Six Categories of Studies Show the Influence
of Father Love
· Studies Looking Exclusively at Variations in
the Influence of Father Love
· Father Love Is as Important as Mother
Love
· Father Love Predicts Specific Outcomes
Better Than Mother Love
· Father Love Is the Sole Significant
Predictor of Specific Outcomes
· Father Love Moderates the Influence of
Mother Love
· Paternal Versus Maternal Parenting May Be
Associated With Different Outcomes in Sons and Daughters
· Discussion
· References
Graphics
· Table 1
· Table 1
· Table 1
Abstract
This article explores the cultural construction of fatherhood in America,
as well as the consequences of this construction as a motivator for
understudying fathers—especially father love—for nearly a century in
developmental and family research. It then reviews evidence from 6 categories of
empirical studies showing the powerful influence of fathers' love on children's
and young adults' social, emotional, and cognitive development and functioning.
Much of this evidence suggests that the influence of father love on offspring's
development is as great as and occasionally greater than the influence of mother
love. Some studies conclude that father love is the sole significant predictor
of specific outcomes after controlling for the influence of mother love.
Overall, father love appears to be as heavily implicated as mother love in
offsprings' psychological well-being and health, as well as in an array of
psychological and behavioral problems.
For most people, life's major satisfactions and pain revolve around
personal relationships with others (Duck, 1988, 1991; Rohner, 1994, 1999). For children, the
most powerful of these others are parents.1 A vast literature shows that the
quality of personal relationships—especially personal relationships with parents
for children—is a major predictor of psychosocial functioning and development
for both children and adults. One dramatically important component of the
concept "quality of relationship" has to do with warmth, supportiveness,
comforting, caring, nurturance, affection, or simply love. In the context of
parent–child relationships, we summarize these elements under the construct
parental acceptance–rejection or, more broadly, under the rubric of the warmth
dimension of parenting (Rohner, 1986,
1999). Four decades of cross-cultural and intracultural research on issues
of parental acceptance–rejection by Rohner (1960, 1975, 1986, 2001) show
that parents anywhere in the world can express their love or lack of love in any
one or a combination of four major ways. Parents can, for example, be warm and
affectionate (or cold and unaffectionate), hostile and aggressive, or
indifferent and neglecting, or they can engage in undifferentiated rejection.
Undifferentiated rejection refers to individuals' affectively charged belief
that their parents do (or did) not really care about them, want them, or love
them, without necessarily having clear behavioral indicators that the parents
are (or were) unaffectionate, aggressive, or neglecting toward them. These and
other such related concepts as parental support, nurturance, closeness, and
caring—concepts that are often used more or less interchangeably by
researchers—are all central elements in the overarching construct of parental
acceptance–rejection or, simply, parental love.
Research in every major ethnic group of America (Rohner, 2001), in dozens of nations
internationally (Khaleque & Rohner,
in press; Rohner & Britner, in
press), and with several hundred societies in two major cross-cultural
comparative samples (Rohner, 1975, 1986,
2001) has shown that children and adults everywhere—regardless of
differences in race, language, gender, or culture—appear to respond in the same
way when they experience themselves to be loved (accepted) or unloved (rejected)
by the people most important to them growing up. The overwhelming bulk of
research dealing with parental acceptance–rejection concentrates on mothers'
behavior, however. Historically, the possible influence of fathers' behavior has
been largely ignored.
This article discusses evidence regarding the relative sparseness of
research on fathers, especially on father love. It then explores the cultural
construction of fatherhood in America and the consequences of this construction
as a principal motivator for overlooking fathers to a large degree for nearly a
century of developmental studies. Finally, it discusses growing evidence about
and implications of the recent recognition of the powerful influence of fathers'
love in child development (Rohner,
1998). Before continuing, we need to specify that this article concentrates
on evidence regarding the influence of fathers' love-related behaviors—or simply
father love—in relation to the social, emotional, and cognitive development and
functioning of children, adolescents, and adult offspring. This emphasis on the
importance of father love should not be construed as minimizing the
well-documented importance of mother love. Rather, it is intended to emphasize
the need to consider the influence of fathers as well as mothers whenever
possible. Finally, where relevant, this article also addresses some of the
antecedents of the effects of fathers' love, including gender and ethnicity.
Other antecedents, including marital quality, separation and divorce, and
children's temperament, for example, have been addressed elsewhere (Booth & Crouter, 1998; Lupton & Barclay, 1997; Phares, 1996, 1997).
Fathers: The Historically Understudied
Little is known about parents' actual behavior within American families
before the 1930s, when empirical research on children and families had its
fullest beginnings. Most of what is known about child rearing before that time
comes from such sources as popular magazines, medical and religious books,
journals, and biographies. These texts tended to exhort parents—almost always
mothers—to behave in a particular way. Or they claimed that parents (mothers)
behaved in a particular way without providing evidence that the claim was true.
In addition, some authors made sweeping but undocumented generalizations about
the effects of maternal (but rarely paternal) behavior. Some, for example, went
so far as to place the entire burden of children's well-being in this life and
the next on mothers' shoulders. In 1849, for instance, Elizabeth Hall wrote in
Mother's Assistant magazine:
Yes, mothers, in a certain sense, the destiny of a redeemed world is put
into your hands; it is for you to say, whether your children shall be
respectable and happy here and prepared for a glorious immortality, or whether
they shall dishonor you, and perhaps bring your grey hairs in sorrow to the
grave, and sink down themselves at last to eternal despair! (p. 27)
Earlier, in the 1700s, Rousseau had already proclaimed that "mothers love
will cure society's ills" (as cited in Kagan, 1978, p. 54).
For more than 200 years, mother love was generally considered paramount
in child development (Kagan, 1978;
Stearns, 1991; Stendler, 1950; Sunley, 1955). Moreover, mothers were
assumed to have nearly exclusive daily responsibility for the care of children.
Fathers were seldom mentioned in the popular press before the mid-1920s (Atkinson & Blackwelder, 1993),
except occasionally as breadwinners, disciplinarians, teachers, and moral
preceptors. Father love was virtually unrecognized by the media. A dramatic
shift occurred with respect to gender-specific parenting articles in the
mid-1920s, however. To illustrate, Atkinson and Blackwelder reviewed a sample of
1,482 popular magazine articles from 1900 to 1989. They found that by the
mid-1920s gender-nonspecific "parenting" articles had begun to reach ascendancy
over "mothering" articles. That is, the term parent began to supplant the term
mother in most articles. It is quite possible, though, as noted by Atkinson and
Blackwelder, that as women began entering the labor force in increasing numbers,
popular writers found it more appropriate to use the term parenting when in fact
they really meant mothering. In any case, only 16% of all articles published
dealt explicitly with fathers. This percentage fluctuated little in the popular
press throughout the course of the 20th century.
This evidence regarding the rarity of reports about fathers and
especially about father love in the popular press is supported by Ellner (1973), who reviewed every
article related to child rearing for the first 6 months of 1950, 1960, and 1970
in three family monthly magazines (Ladies Home Journal, Good Housekeeping, and
Parents Magazine). Of the 177 articles reviewed, only 3 dealt with fathers. In
these, fathers were urged to participate in the discipline of their children and
in children's sex education. Beyond that, fathers were viewed as important role
models for sons and as husband models for daughters. None of the articles dealt
with father love.
Early professional publications in child development and family studies
reflect similar trends to those found in the popular press. Peterson, Becker, Hellmer, Shoemaker, and
Quay (1959), for example, reviewed the professional literature on
parent–child relations from 1929 to 1956. They found about 169 publications
dealing with mother–child relationships but only 12 (7%) dealing with
father–child relationships. Eron, Banta,
Walder, and Laulicht (1961) supported this conclusion when they estimated
that about 15 times as many publications dealt with mother–child relations as
with father–child relations. Furthermore, they found that when information was
collected about fathers, it was commonly obtained from men's wives, not from
fathers themselves. Nash (1965)
argued that this tendency to obtain information about fathers from mothers (or
from children) resulted from the researchers' implicit assumption that fathers
themselves were inaccessible because of out-of-home economic responsibilities.
It is difficult to determine from these early reviews of professional
literature what percentage of the few studies that dealt with fathers also
implicated father love in some way. An estimate may be made, however, from the
work of Rohner and Nielsen (1978).
These authors completed a critical review of the literature dealing with
parental acceptance–rejection from about 1930 through 1976. In their work they
found about 600 relevant studies. Of these, 108 (17%) mentioned fathers, but 70%
of the latter references occurred in the 1960s and 1970s, a time when many other
researchers were also recognizing fathers as significant parenting figures. Phares and Compas (1992), for example,
reviewed every article in eight clinical and child and adolescent journals from
1984 through 1991. The authors wanted to ascertain possible gender bias in the
reporting of parental influences in child and adolescent psychopathology. They
found that 48% of the articles reviewed included only mothers, whereas 1%
included only fathers. However, 26% of the studies obtained and analyzed data
separately for both mothers and fathers. These figures are representative of the
fact that a virtual revolution has occurred since the 1960s and 1970s in the
recognition of fathers, albeit only minor recognition of the influence of father
love (Biller, 1974, 1981, 1993; Hanson & Bozett, 1985, 1991; Hewlett, 1992; Lamb, 1975, 1981, 1986, 1997; Mackey, 1996; Radin, 1981). We turn now to an
exploration of the following question: Why has father love been so understudied
for nearly a century in research on parent–child relations?
Fatherhood Is a Cultural Construction
Though usually unintended and often unrecognized, much of behavioral
science is a value-laden enterprise (Kaplan, 1964; Silverstein & Auerbach, 1999).
Research questions that are regarded as appropriate or sensible at a particular
point in time are usually situated within a matrix of cultural beliefs often
widely accepted within the dominant population at large, but certainly within
the scientific community more specifically. The issue of fatherhood is a case in
point. Fatherhood is a cultural construction (Doherty, Kouneski, & Erickson,
1998), and once formulated it has implications for the subsequent behavior
of those who share the beliefs and assumptions defining that construction.
The issue here is to understand the meanings commonly associated in the
United States with the concepts of father, fatherhood, and fathering and to
recognize the constraints that are implicitly placed on one's behaviors as a
result of accepting these meanings (i.e., cultural construction) as being true.
Of course, to understand fully the cultural construction of fatherhood, one must
also understand its counterpoint, motherhood. And one must also understand that
both constructions are influenced by cultural conceptions of masculinity and
femininity. It is not our intention here, however, to disentangle the maze of
meanings and behavioral implications of all of these conceptions and
relationships, but merely to acknowledge that they exist. For now we want only
to dwell on the cultural meaning of fatherhood in America in relation to
motherhood and to explore the implications of this conception for behavioral
science research on parent–child relationships.
For most Americans, the concepts father, fatherhood, and fathering appear
to connote very different domains of behavior and affect from the concepts
mother, motherhood, and mothering. Semantically, the cultural construction of
"fathering," for example, implies nothing about father love as does the
genderized equivalent of mother love that is contained semantically in the
construct "mothering." The word mothering elicits, for many, a warm, fuzzy,
nurtured feeling, whereas the term fathering elicits feelings of something
stronger, colder, harder, and less affectionate (Rohner, 1995). The term father love is
not used in everyday discourse, but mother love is. Even the phrase "father
love" sounds strange to some, yet many feel comforted by "mother love" (Rohner, 1995). Popular literature is
filled with references to both mothering and parenting when referring to
caregiving. But the term fathering is almost never used in this context. When
used, the term is typically found in the context of the question "Who begot
whom?" Even the gender-neutral term parent is often used or interpreted as being
synonymous with mother. And we have found in our own teaching that
students—including advanced graduate students—often misread and even mishear the
term paternal as being parental, which is then sometimes translated as
maternal/mother.
In some respects, the conception of fatherhood has shifted dramatically
over the course of the last 300 years of American history (Lamb, 2000). According to E. H. Pleck and Pleck (1997), for example,
the ideal image of the colonial European American father in the 1600s and 1700s
was that of the stern patriarch. From 1830 to 1900 the ideal image of father was
one of the distant breadwinner. From 1900 to 1970 the ideal father was the
genial playmate dad and gender role model. And from the 1970s to today the ideal
image of father is said to be one who is a co-parent, sharing equally with his
mate in the care of their children. This portrayal of the cultural conception of
the ideal father is greatly oversimplified, of course, but it does contain
important ideas that have been widely shared, and it shows how cultural
conceptions of fatherhood have shifted over time.
One of the most enduring historical elements defining fatherhood has
nothing to do with rearing children but deals with the assumption that the major
role of fathers within the family is as economic provider: the breadwinner (La Rossa, 1997). Throughout much of the
20th century and earlier—beginning with the advent of industrialization in the
19th century—many American men judged themselves (and were judged by others),
judged their personal worth, and judged their success as husbands and fathers in
relation to their ability to provide economically for their families (Stearns, 1991). Since the 1940s, this
essential role has been deemphasized somewhat, although it has not been
altogether abandoned or replaced. The shared ideology of male breadwinning was
used by many researchers over the course of the 20th century to explain fathers'
apparently limited involvement in child care (Griswold, 1993).
More important, however, many behavioral scientists prior to the 1960s
and 1970s assumed that fathers were relatively unimportant for the healthy
development of their children in any case (Cabrera, Tamis-LeMonda, Bradley, Hofferth,
& Lamb, 2000; Nash, 1965; Rapoport, Rapoport, Strelitz, & Kew,
1977). At the very most, fathers were thought to be peripheral to the job of
parenting because children spent most of their time with their mothers. Some
even argued that fathers have no biological aptitude for child care, though
women were said to be genetically endowed for it (Amato, 1998; Belsky, 1998; Benson, 1968). These conceptions seemed
to have led in the popular press and in many television portrayals during the
1950s and 1960s to an image of the irrelevant, mindless, ineffectual, sometimes
bumbling and incompetent father (Mackey,
1996). Not all literary and visual portrayals of fathers were negative,
however. For example, movies and television series such as Make Room for Daddy,
Father Knows Bes, The Cosby Show, and others portrayed warm, loving, involved,
and competent fathers. Similarly, some 19th- and 20th-century novels described
loving, concerned, and competent fathers. Bob Cratchit in Dickens's (1843) A Christmas Carol and
Atticus Finch in Lee's (1960) To
Kill a Mockingbird are two cases in point. And even though comic strip fathers
were sometimes lampooned in the 20th century as being bumbling incompetents, La Rossa, Jaret, Gadgil, and Wynn
(2000) also showed that these characters were frequently portrayed from 1940
to 1999 as being nurturant, supportive, and capable fathers.
The cultural conception of fathers as being a relatively inconsequential
influence in child development has its counterpoint in an assumption about the
primacy of mothers and mother love. We commented earlier on the latter issue.
Now we amplify on the topic by pointing out that virtually all of the
influential theories of child development during the past century accepted the
unproven premise that mothers are most important in child development. For
example, psychoanalytic theory specifically and psychodynamic theories more
generally assume that the mother–child relationship—especially during the first
6 years of life—is crucial for normal child development. Early attachment theory
too focused on the mother–child bond, as did significant portions of learning
theory and cognitive developmental theory (Lamb, 1975, 1981, 1986; Phares, 1992, 1996). With this phalanx
of distinguished theories converging on a view that was widely believed in
America anyway, few developmental researchers in the first half of the 20th
century thought it essential to inquire directly about fathers' influence in
child development. Shared wisdom asserted that competent and nurturant mothering
was all children really needed for successful cognitive, social, and emotional
development (Bronstein & Cowan,
1988). Or, as one distinguished researcher wrote in an anonymous review of
this article: "We spent a lot of time studying mothers because we thought they
were important, not because we thought fathers weren't."
Cultural assumptions such as these about fathering are related to widely
shared assumptions about appropriate gender role and gender identity of both men
and women. In this regard, most Americans—men as well as women—seem to have
associated child care with feminine behavior. Until a decade or so ago, many
Americans regarded it as unmasculine for men to spend much time caring for
children, except as a temporary backup and support to the mothers. A common
theoretical assumption found even as recently as the 1970s and 1980s asserted
that fathers' major contribution to child development was indirect, through
their economic and emotional support of mothers (Biller, 1993; Maccoby & Martin, 1983). This
premise may be summarized as follows: The most important contribution a father
can make to his children is achieved through providing for and loving their
mother. In summary, the widely held cultural construction of fatherhood prior to
the 1970s (and still held by many today) has two strands. One deals with
fathers' competence as caregivers, and the other deals with the influence of
fathering on child development. The first strand asserts that fathers are often
incompetent and maybe even biologically unsuited to the job of child rearing.
(The maternal counterpoint is that women are genetically endowed for child
care.) The second strand asserts that fathers' influence in child development is
relatively unimportant or at least peripheral or indirect. (The maternal
counterpoint is that mother love and competent maternal care provide everything
that children need for normal, healthy development.)
The effect of internalizing these cultural beliefs as one's own personal
beliefs seems to have led to sometimes unintended and unrecognized but
nonetheless real consequences. The most notable outcome of acting on these
beliefs throughout most of the 20th century was to minimize fathers' presence in
much of mainstream behavioral science research as well as in clinical research
(Phares, 1997). Because it was
assumed that mothers but not necessarily fathers were important in child
development, researchers tended to study mostly mothers' behavior. Of course,
they found significant effects of maternal behavior that served to motivate
researchers even further to study mothers. But a subtle side effect of these
results also seemed to reinforce researchers' belief that fathers must not be
all that important because mothers were being shown to be so very important.
Moreover, behavioral scientists and clinical practitioners felt further
justified in excluding fathers from their work because mothers spent the
greatest amount of time with children, and therefore it was reasoned that they
must also have the greatest influence on children's development as well as on
treatment outcomes (Ferholt &
Gurwitt, 1982; Phares, 1996).
Finally, fathers tended to be omitted from research and treatment models because
they were assumed to be inaccessible as a result of their out-of-home economic
responsibilities.
The relatively little research that included fathers prior to 1970 dealt
with a variety of issues, but seldom with father love per se. This omission
seems to have been encouraged by the widespread "tendency among both researchers
and theorists to accept without question the assumption that fathers express
less affection and understanding toward children than do mothers" (Walters & Stinnett, 1971, p. 102).
Indeed, many behavioral scientists and clinicians seemed to accept the postulate
that fathers' major role in the family was in the instrumental domain, whereas
mothers' major role was in the expressive–affective domain (Parsons & Bales, 1955). Acceptance
of these assumptions resulted in very limited research specifically examining
the relationship between father love and child outcomes.
Growing Awareness of the Influence of Father Love
Though it was not until the 1960s that researchers began to find with any
regularity that father love was as predictive as mother love of children's
psychological and behavioral adjustment, occasional note was made of this fact
as early as the 1940s. R. W. Lidz and
Lidz (1949), for example, claimed that faulty paternal influences were as
common as maternal influences in the development of child psychopathology.
Later, T. Lidz, Parker, and Cornelison
(1956) claimed that domineering, sadistic, and rejecting fathers were more
implicated in the etiology of schizophrenia than were mothers. Peterson et al. (1959) completed one of
the first studies examining the attitudes of both fathers and mothers and their
effects on both disturbed and normal children. From this research, the authors
noted that "contrary to general assumption and our own original expectation, the
attitudes of fathers were found to be at least as intimately related as the
attitudes of mothers to the occurrence and form of maladjustive tendencies among
children" (p. 129).
Empirical evidence such as this about the strong influence of father love
led Becker, Peterson, Hellmer,
Shoemaker, and Quay (1959) and Becker (1960) to make an emphatic call
for more systematic study of the role of fathers in child development. And
later, in his classic review of the literature on parent–child relations, Becker (1964) wrote, "where both
mothers and fathers have been studied, most of the research has shown the
father's influence on the child's behavior to be at least equal to that of the
mother" (p. 204). For the most part, this early evidence and call for additional
research about the possible influence of fathers' behavior was ignored because
the then current cultural ideology continued to endorse the
primacy-of-the-mother doctrine.
Nearly a decade later, in a major review of the 1960s parent–child
relationship literature, Walters and
Stinnett (1971) reiterated these observations:
Because we have believed that the impact of mothers upon the development
of children is greater than the impact of fathers, we have investigated maternal
impact to a far greater extent than we have examined the impact of fathers. Yet,
much of the evidence of the past decade suggests that the variability of
children's behavior is more closely associated with the type of father one has
than the type of mother [italics added]. (p. 102)
Other early sources of evidence about the importance of fathering and
father love also went more or less unheeded because of the cultural bias in
America emphasizing the singular importance of mothers. For example,
anthropologists have documented repeatedly the fact that women the world over
tend to be the major caregivers of children, but they are not the exclusive
caregivers (Mackey, 1996; Rohner & Rohner, 1981). Fathers,
siblings, grandparents, and others are common substitutes. As early as 1956 the
noted anthropologist, Margaret Mead, concluded that "anthropological evidence
gives no support … to the value of such an accentuation of the tie between
mother and child. On the contrary, cross-cultural studies suggest that
adjustment is most facilitated if the child is cared for by many warm, friendly
people" (pp. 642–643). Fathers often rank among the most significant of these
others.
In the same general domain, results from Rohner's (1975) early research on the
worldwide antecedents and correlates of parental acceptance–rejection also went
mostly unheeded. More specifically, in a cross-cultural comparative study of 101
societies representing a stratified sample of the known and adequately described
sociocultural systems of the world, Rohner found that children everywhere—across
the full range of the world's economic systems, political systems, household
types, and other sociocultural factors—tend to be accepted by mothers and other
major caregivers to a greater degree in households where fathers are present on
a day-to-day basis than in households where fathers are present less often.
Moreover, results of this research showed that the more important fathers are as
socializing agents in relation to other caregivers such as mothers, siblings,
and grandparents, the greater the warmth children receive from all major
caregivers in that society. In addition, the more time fathers willingly spend
tending their offspring in relation to other caregivers, the more likely
children are to be accepted. These results were replicated but essentially
ignored in a subsequent holocultural study of 186 societies worldwide (Rohner, 1986; Rohner & Rohner, 1982).
Acknowledging the Importance of Fathers and Father Love
Gradually, many behavioral scientists began to acknowledge that fathers
should not be overlooked in child development and family studies, and especially
in studies of parental acceptance–rejection. The inception and at times almost
surprised and grudging recognition of this fact began in the 1960s and 1970s
because of the convergence of three sources of influence. First, as we noted
earlier, spotty evidence was already mounting about the importance of fathers
and father love, but behavioral scientists had been able to ignore the evidence
fairly successfully until later. Second, the advent of the feminist movement
with its call for gender equality in the workplace and partner equity in the
home led eventually to a reexamination of the cultural construction of feminine
gender identity. Reexamination of the meaning of femininity soon called for a
reexamination of its counterpoint: masculine gender identity.
This interest in gender role redefinition was fueled by the fact that
mothers with children were entering the out-of-home workforce in unprecedented
numbers. In 1960, for example, fewer than 40% of American mothers worked out of
the home, but by 1972 almost 50% were doing so (Biller, 1993). By 1997, 68% of the
mothers in the United States with children less than 18 years old were engaged
in full-time or part-time employment (Child Trends, 2000). As greater numbers
of mothers began working, the feminist movement called for fathers to become
more involved in child care and household tasks (Biller, 1993; Bronstein & Cowan, 1988; Griswold, 1993). To be successfully
accomplished, these changes required a partial redefinition of traditional male
and female gender roles (Jain, Belsky
& Crnic, 1996; Mackey,
1996). This in turn brought the role of fathering and the influence of
father love into sharp research focus (Maccoby & Martin, 1983). For the
first time, some behavioral scientists became keenly motivated to study fathers
directly (Bronstein & Cowan,
1988; Giveans & Robinson,
1985; Parke, 1985).
It is not our purpose here to try to fully disentangle the snarled web of
competing social, political, economic, and cultural forces associated with the
feminist movement that eventually politicized fatherhood in America (Griswold, 1993). Here we simply want to
recognize that as a direct as well as indirect result of the feminist movement,
many behavioral scientists began to study fathers and father love directly. And
when they did, they found that fathers are as capable as mothers of being
competent and nurturant caregivers (Bronstein & Cowan, 1988; Silverstein & Auerbach, 1999). They
also found that the father–child bond often parallels the mother–child bond both
emotionally and in intensity (Fox,
Kimmerly, & Schafer, 1991; Hanson & Bozett, 1991).
A number of the studies reviewed subsequently continued to show, as in
earlier decades, that fathers' influence was as great as mothers' for specific
developmental outcomes. But it was not until the 1990s that the third source of
influence came into full play, causing many behavioral scientists to fully
recognize that fathers should be included in studies dealing with parent–child
relations. This source of influence derived from research results based on
readily accessible, easily used, and powerful multivariate statistical packages.
Use of multivariate statistics, including multiple regression and structural
equation modeling (SEM), allowed investigators to control simultaneously for the
influence of a variety of variables. In doing so, researchers discovered that
father love sometimes explains a unique, independent portion of the variance in
specific child outcomes, over and above the portion of variance explained by
mother love. Indeed, some studies reviewed later found that father love is the
sole significant predictor of specific child outcomes after removing the
influence of mother love.
Six Categories of Studies Show the Influence of Father Love
Six categories of empirical studies show the influence of father love on
specific child outcomes: (a) Some studies look exclusively at the influence of
variations of father love without examining the influence of mother love too;
(b) some conclude that father love is equally as important as mother love in
predicting specific child outcomes; (c) some conclude that father love predicts
specific child outcomes better than mother love; (d) some conclude that father
love is the sole significant predictor of specific child outcomes after removing
the influence of mother love; (e) some conclude that father love moderates (Baron & Kenny, 1986) the influence
of mother love on specific child outcomes; and, finally, (f) some conclude that
paternal versus maternal parenting may be associated with a single outcome or
with different outcomes in sons and daughters. These six categories emerged
naturalistically from the growing body of empirical research dealing with the
influence of father love. Three of the categories (the second through fourth)
are especially noteworthy because they show that father love continues to make a
unique and significant contribution to child outcomes after statistically
controling for the influence of mother love.
We briefly review each of these six categories of evidence. Of course,
many other aspects of fathers' behavior, some of which may be closely related to
father love—such as father involvement, father absence, and fathers'
psychological and behavioral state—also influence child development. Here,
though, we focus on the impact of father love (or paternal acceptance–rejection)
per se because, as studies reviewed subsequently show, father love by itself is
implicated in a wide array of developmental issues. These include youths'
psychological adjustment, behavior problems, delinquency, gender role
development, cognitive/academic/intellectual development, achievement, and
social competence. Moreover, father love has also been shown to be associated
with children's and adults' psychological health and sense of well-being. Even
though some of these studies conclude that father love is the sole significant
predictor of specific outcomes—after controlling for the influence of mother
love—none suggest that mother love is unimportant in other contexts. We should
note here that authors of the articles reviewed subsequently use a variety of
terms to discuss different aspects of the father love (paternal
acceptance–rejection) construct. Many of these concepts, such as paternal
warmth, nurturance, support, caring, and affection, are used more or less
interchangeably and synonymously with paternal acceptance–rejection. Because of
this, we generally retain the major terms used by the various authors. Sample
characteristics of the empirical studies cited in this article are provided in
Table 1.
Table 1 Sample Characteristics of Empirical Studies Cited
Table 1 (continued)
Table 1 (continued)
Studies Looking Exclusively at Variations in the Influence of Father Love
Many studies looking exclusively at the influence of variations in father
love deal with two topics: (a) gender role development (Fish & Biller, 1973; Green, 1982; Huttenen, 1992; Millen & Roll, 1997; Musser & Fleck, 1983) and (b)
father involvement (Amato & Rivera,
1999; Biller, 1993; K. N. Harris, Furstenberg, & Marmer,
1998; Radin, 1981). Studies of
gender role development emerged prominently in the 1940s and continued through
the 1970s (Lamb, 1997, 2000). This
was a time when fathers were considered to be especially important as gender
role models for sons. Commonly, researchers assessed the masculinity of fathers
and of sons and then correlated the two sets of scores. Many behavioral
scientists were surprised to discover that no consistent results emerged from
this research until they examined the quality of the father–son relationship.
Then they found that when the relationship between masculine fathers and their
sons was warm and loving, the boys were indeed more masculine. Later, however,
researchers found that the masculinity of fathers per se did not seem to make
much difference after all.2 As
summarized by Lamb (1997):
Boys seemed to conform to the sex-role standards of their culture when
their relationships with their fathers were warm, regardless of how "masculine"
the fathers were, even though warmth and intimacy have traditionally been seen
as feminine characteristics. A similar conclusion was suggested by research on
other aspects of psychosocial adjustment and on achievement: Paternal warmth or
closeness appeared beneficial, whereas paternal masculinity appeared irrelevant.
(p. 9)
The second domain in which a substantial amount of research has been done
on the influence of variations in father love deals with father involvement,
that is, with the amount of time that fathers spend with their children
(engagement), the extent to which fathers make themselves available to their
children (accessibility), and the extent to which they take responsibility for
their children's care and welfare (responsibility; Lamb, Pleck, Chernov, & Levine,
1987). Many studies conclude that children with highly involved fathers, in
relation to children with less involved fathers, tend to be more cognitively and
socially competent, less inclined toward gender stereotyping, more empathic, and
psychologically better adjusted (Biller,
1981, 1993; Easterbrooks &
Goldberg, 1984; Lamb, 1997; J.
H. Pleck, 1997; Radin, 1981; Radin & Russell, 1983; Radin & Sagi, 1982; Radin, Williams, & Coggins, 1993;
Reuter & Biller, 1973; E. Williams & Radin, 1993; S. Williams & Finley, 1997). Commonly,
these studies investigate both paternal warmth and paternal involvement and
find—using simple correlations—that the two variables are related to each other
and to youth outcomes.
It is unclear from these studies whether involvement and warmth make
independent or joint contributions to youth outcomes. Moreover, "caring for"
children is not necessarily the same thing as "caring about" them. Indeed, Lamb (1997) concluded from his review
of studies of paternal involvement that it was not the simple fact of paternal
engagement (i.e., direct interaction with the child), availability, or
responsibility for child care that was associated with these outcomes. Rather,
it appears that the quality of the father–child relationship made the greatest
difference (Cabrera et al., 2000; Lamb, 1997). J. H. Pleck (1997) reiterated this conclusion
when he wrote:
The critical question is: How good is the evidence that fathers' amount
of involvement, without taking into account its content and quality, is
consequential for children, mothers, or fathers themselves? The associations
with desirable outcomes found in much research are actually with positive forms
of paternal involvement, not involvement per se. Involvement needs to be
combined with qualitative dimensions of paternal behavior through the concept of
"positive paternal involvement" developed here. (pp. 66–67)
Research by Veneziano and Rohner
(1998) supports these conclusions. In a biracial sample of 63 African
American and European American children, the authors found from multiple
regression analyses that father involvement by itself was associated with
children's psychological adjustment primarily insofar as it was perceived by
youths to be an expression of paternal warmth (acceptance). These results varied
by ethnicity, however. In the European American families, fathers' loving
acceptance significantly mediated (Baron
& Kenny, 1986) the way offspring experienced their fathers' involvement.
In the African American families, however, father involvement made no
significant contribution to youths' psychological adjustment, but perceived
paternal warmth (acceptance) did. Neither parenting nor youths' psychological
adjustment varied significantly, however, by social class.
Further evidence in support of Pleck's conclusion about the importance of
paternal warmth versus levels of paternal involvement is shown in Veneziano's (1998) cross-cultural
comparative, holocultural study. Using multiple regression analysis, in a sample
of 32 societies representing the world's known and adequately described
sociocultural systems, he found that the lack of paternal warmth—not the amount
of time that fathers were involved with children— predicted young males'
interpersonal violence.
Finally, Amato and Gilbreth's
(1999) meta-analysis of 63 studies that explored the relationship between
nonresidential fathering and children's well-being showed that paternal
encouragement, support, and closeness were more predictive of youths' social,
emotional, and psychological well-being than the frequency of contact between
children and their nonresident fathers.
Father Love Is as Important as Mother Love
As we indicated earlier, many of the studies concluding that father love
is as influential as mother love go back to the 1940s. Most of these
conclusions, especially those prior to the 1980s, are drawn from correlational
studies in which the simple correlation between a specific measure of paternal
love and a specific child outcome is as great as or greater than the simple
correlation between the same measure of maternal love and the child outcome.
More recently, however, the 1980s and 1990s saw behavioral scientists use forms
of multivariate analyses that allowed them to conclude that both fathers' and
mothers' behaviors are associated significantly and uniquely with specific child
outcomes.
The great majority of studies showing that father love is as important as
mother love deal with one or a combination of the following five issues among
children, adolescents, and young adults: (a) personality and psychological
adjustment problems including issues of self-concept/self-esteem, emotional
stability, and aggression (Amato,
1998; Becker, 1960; Becker et al., 1959; Buri, 1989; Buri, Louiselle, Misukanis, & Mueller,
1988; Buri, Murphy, Richtsmeier,
& Komar, 1992; Dekovic &
Meeus, 1997; Emmelkamp &
Karsdorp, 1987; Fine, Voydanoff,
& Donnelly, 1993; Jacobs,
Spilken, & Norman, 1972; McPherson, 1974; Monkman, 1958; Nash, 1965; Peppin, 1962; Peterson et al., 1959; Peterson, Becker, Shoemaker, Luria, &
Hellmer, 1961; Sears, 1970; Yamasaki, 1990); (b) conduct problems,
especially in school (Becker, 1960;
DeKlyen, Biernbaum, Speltz, &
Greenberg, 1998; DeKlyen, Speltz,
& Greenberg, 1998; McPherson,
1974; Paley, Conger, & Harold,
2000; Patterson, Reid, &
Dishion, 1992; Renk, Phares, &
Epps, 1999; Russell & Russell,
1996; Siantz & Smith, 1994);
(c) cognitive and academic performance issues (Amato, 1998; Carroll, 1973; Easterbrooks & Goldberg, 1984; Heilbrun, Orr, & Harrell, 1966; Peppin, 1962); (d) mental illness (Arrindell, Emmelkamp, Monsma, &
Brilman, 1983; Crook, Raskin, &
Eliot, 1981; Greenberger & Chen,
1996; R. W. Lidz & Lidz,
1949; T. Lidz et al., 1956; Richter, Richter, & Eisemann,
1990); and (e) substance abuse (Barnes, 1984; Prendergast & Schaefer, 1974).
Only newer studies in the 1980s and 1990s used complex multivariate
analyses to systematically test for the relative influence of fathers' love in
contrast to mothers' love. The work of Young, Miller, Norton, and Hill (1995)
illustrates this. These authors drew from a national sample of 640 adolescents
12 to 16 years old living in two-parent families. Employing SEM techniques, they
found that perceived paternal love and caring were as predictive of sons' and
daughters' life satisfaction—including their sense of well-being—as maternal
love and caring. Review of a broad range of studies such as these led Lamb (1997) to conclude that
fathers and mothers seem to influence their children in similar rather
than dissimilar ways. Contrary to the expectations of many psychologists,
including myself, who have studied paternal influences on children, the
differences between mothers and fathers appear much less important than the
similarities. Students of socialization have consistently found that parental
warmth, nurturance, and closeness are associated with positive child outcomes
whether the parent or adult involved is a mother or a father. The important
dimensions of parental influence are those that have to do with parental
characteristics rather than gender-related characteristics. (p. 13)
Father Love Predicts Specific Outcomes Better Than Mother Love
Two types of studies are common in this category. First, results of some
bivariate correlational studies have led researchers to conclude that fathers'
love is more strongly associated than mothers' love with specific child
behaviors such as those noted subsequently. Second, the 1980s and especially the
1990s saw a proliferation of studies using multiple regression and SEM. As these
analytic procedures became more commonplace, it also became more common to
discover that the influence of father love explains a unique, independent
portion of the variance in specific child outcomes—detailed subsequently—over
and above the portion of variance explained by mother love.
Studies in this category tend to deal with one or more of the following
six issues among children, adolescents, and young adults: (a) personality and
psychological adjustment problems (Amato, 1994; Dominy, Johnson, & Koch, 2000; Komarovsky, 1976; Stagner, 1933; Tacon & Caldera, 2001), (b) conduct
problems (Chen, Liu, & Li, 2000;
Eron et al., 1961; Grant et al., 2000), (c) delinquency
(Andry, 1962), (d) mental illness
(Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992;
Lefkowitz & Tesiny, 1984), (e)
substance abuse (Brook & Brook,
1988; Emmelkamp & Heeres,
1988), and (f) psychological health and well-being (Amato, 1994). We briefly review an
example of each issue.
Research by Dominy et al.
(2000) illustrates the first issue. In this work, the authors studied the
relation between perceived childhood experiences of parental (maternal and
paternal) acceptance–rejection and the presence or absence of binge eating
disorder (BED) among a group of 113 women. Eighty-three of these women were
obese. Of these, 32 suffered from BED. Thirty women, however, neither were obese
nor had an eating disorder. The authors found that women with BED perceived
their fathers to be significantly more rejecting than did women in either of the
other two groups. In addition, the women with BED perceived their fathers to be
significantly more rejecting than their mothers.
A 2-year longitudinal study of 258 sixth-grade students (at Time 1) in
the People's Republic of China serves to illustrate the second issue: dealing
with conduct problems. In this study Chen et al. (2000) found that paternal
but not maternal warmth at Time 1 was negatively associated with youths'
disruptive aggression toward peers at Time 2. In addition, they found that
paternal but not maternal warmth was positively associated with children's
academic performance and social competence as judged by teachers.
An older work by Andry (1962)
illustrates the third type of study: the link between paternal and maternal
rejection and delinquency. Here Andry found in a matched sample of 80 delinquent
boys 11 through 15 years of age that the great majority of delinquents felt
rejected by their fathers but not necessarily by their mothers. Nondelinquents,
on the other hand, tended to feel loved by both parents. Of special interest in
Andry's study is the fact that fathers as well as mothers in the two groups
tended to corroborate the youths' perceptions of parenting.
Research by Rohner and Brothers
(1999) illustrates the fourth issue, dealing with the link between parenting
and mental illness. These authors examined the relation between perceived
parental (paternal and maternal) acceptance–rejection and self-reported
psychological adjustment in a sample of 17 women diagnosed as suffering from
borderline personality disorder. This group was compared with a control sample
of 18 nonclinical women. Results showed that women in the clinical group tended
to perceive qualitatively more paternal rejection than acceptance in their
families of origin. Perceived maternal rejection was also elevated somewhat, but
not to the point where these women experienced qualitatively more maternal
rejection than acceptance. And, as is generally true throughout much of America
(Rohner, 1986, 2001), most of the
women in the nonclinical control group tended to perceive substantial paternal
as well as maternal love and overall acceptance.
Research by Campo and Rohner
(1992) illustrates the fifth issue, dealing with the relation between
parenting and substance abuse. These researchers studied 40 drug abusers in
relation to a control sample of 40 nonabusers. On the average, the
polydrug-addicted group—both males and females—had experienced qualitatively
more paternal rejection than acceptance before the onset of serious drug use as
adolescents. The substance abusers had also experienced significant love
withdrawal at the hands of their mothers, but not to the point of having
experienced qualitatively more rejection than acceptance. Participants in the
nonabuse group, on the other hand, had experienced substantial paternal as well
as maternal love and acceptance in their families of origin. Discriminant
function analysis showed that perceived paternal acceptance–rejection,
self-reported psychological adjustment, perceived maternal acceptance–rejection,
and level of education (in that order of importance) predicted with 91.2%
accuracy who among the 80 participants were drug abusers versus nonabusers.
Perceptions of father love and love withdrawal were overwhelmingly the best
single predictor.
Finally, Amato's (1994) work
serves to illustrate the sixth issue. Here, Amato showed that father love is
sometimes implicated to a greater extent than mother love in adult offsprings'
overall psychological health and well-being. Drawing from telephone interviews
with a national sample of 471 young adults, he found that perceived closeness to
fathers for both sons and daughters made a unique contribution—over and above
the contribution made by perceived closeness to mothers—to adult offsprings'
happiness, life satisfaction, and low psychological distress (i.e., overall
psychological well-being). In the author's words, "Regardless of the quality of
the mother-child relationship, the closer adult offspring were to their fathers,
the happier, more satisfied, and less distressed they reported being" (p. 1039).
Father Love Is the Sole Significant Predictor of Specific Outcomes
A growing number of studies using a variety of multivariate statistics
have begun to conclude that father love is occasionally the sole significant
predictor of specific child outcomes after removing the influence of mother
love. Studies in this category tend to deal most often with one or more of the
following three issues among children, adolescents, and young adults: (a)
personality and psychological adjustment problems (Barnett, Marshall, & Pleck, 1992;
Bartle, Anderson, & Sabatelli,
1989; Dickie et al., 1997; DuBos, Eitel, & Felner, 1994; Matsuda & Ritblatt, 1998; Wagner & Philips, 1992), (b)
conduct and delinquency problems (Kroupa, 1988), and (c) substance abuse
(Brook, Whiteman, & Gordon,
1981; Eldred, Brown, & Mahabir,
1974).
Cole and McPherson (1993),
for example, concluded from their SEM analysis of 107 adolescents and parents
that father–child conflict but not mother–child conflict (each controlling for
the other) was positively associated with adolescent depressive symptoms.
Moreover, father–adolescent cohesion was positively associated with the absence
of adolescent depressive symptoms. These results are consistent with Barrera and Garrison-Jones's (1992)
conclusion that adolescents' satisfaction with paternal support was related to
adolescent depression, whereas maternal support was not. A similar study by Barnett et al. (1992) was conducted
among 285 married adult sons with two living parents. When measures of the
quality of both mother–son and father–son relationships were entered
simultaneously into a regression equation, only the father–son relationship was
significantly related to adult sons' psychological distress (a summed measure of
anxiety and depression).
In a sample of 70 middle- to lower-middle-class adolescents, Forehand and Nousiainen (1993) assessed
relations between school functioning, as evaluated by teachers, and youths'
perceptions of parental (paternal and maternal) acceptance–rejection. Using
stepwise multiple regression, the authors found that teachers' ratings of
youths' social competence and conduct problems were associated with the
adolescents' perceptions of paternal but not maternal acceptance.
A 6-year longitudinal study begun in 1981 by Brody, Moore, and Glei (1994) showed
that paternal (but not maternal) warmth had a significant long-term effect in
shaping adolescents' attitudes in 1987 toward such social issues as marriage,
divorce, sex roles, child support, welfare, and teenage childbearing. More
specifically, the warmer fathers were and the more adolescents were allowed to
participate in family decision making in 1981, the more adolescents internalized
their parents' values over time as being their own. This relationship was true
only for warm father–adolescent relationships. Warm mother–adolescent
relationships had no significant effect on youths' later attitudes. Results from
this study were based on 592 families participating in a nationally
representative household survey of 11- to 16-year-old youths.
Finally, Veneziano (2000)
found in a sample of 281 African American and European American families that
only paternal warmth was significantly related to the European American youths'
psychological adjustment when controlling for the influence of maternal warmth.
Indeed, maternal warmth dropped from the regression model altogether. However,
in the African American families, paternal as well as maternal warmth was
significantly related to youths' psychological adjustment, making both
independent and joint contributions.
Father Love Moderates the Influence of Mother Love
Fathers' behavior may moderate and be moderated by—that is, interact with
(Baron & Kenny, 1986; Hull, Tedlie, & Zahn, 1992)—other
influences within the family. Apparently, however, only one study so far has
addressed the issue of mother love having different effects on specific child
outcomes depending on the level of father love. Here, Forehand and Nousiainen (1993) found in
a sample of 70 adolescents and their parents that the relative level of fathers'
love and acceptance moderated the contribution of mothers' loving acceptance to
adolescent functioning. Specifically, variation in father love contributed to
adolescents' cognitive competence as well as to anxiety–withdrawal
(internalizing problems) through its interaction with maternal acceptance.
Regarding cognitive competence, for example, when mothers were high in
acceptance but fathers were low, teachers judged youths' cognitive competence to
be quite low, lower even than when mothers' acceptance was also low. But when
fathers' loving acceptance was high, mothers' loving acceptance was associated
with the most positive levels of cognitive competence. From this study, the
authors drew the important inference that "simply including fathers in
parent-adolescent research is insufficient. Instead, the potential ways in which
each parent's style contributes to the other parent's style must be considered"
(p. 219).
Paternal Versus Maternal Parenting May Be Associated With Different
Outcomes in Sons and Daughters
Two types of research tend to be found in this category. First, some
research shows that one pattern of paternal behavior and a different pattern of
maternal behavior is associated with a single outcome in sons, daughters, or
sometimes both offspring. Second, other research in this category shows that a
single pattern of paternal love-related behavior is associated with one outcome
for sons and a different outcome for daughters. We briefly review examples of
both types.
The work of Barber and Thomas
(1986) illustrates the first type. These authors found in a sample of 527
adolescents that daughters' self-esteem was best predicted by fathers' physical
affection (kisses and hugs) and by mothers' general support, including maternal
praise, approval, encouragement, use of terms of endearment, and helping
behaviors. Sons' self-esteem, on the other hand, was best predicted by fathers'
sustained contact (e.g., picking up the boy for fun and safety) and by mothers'
companionship (i.e., spending time with the boy and sharing activities with
him). In addition, in perhaps the earliest study ever done of this type, Fitz-Simmons (1935) found that paternal
rejection and maternal overprotection characterized the parenting styles among
96 emotionally withdrawn children being treated in child guidance clinics.
Moreover, a substantial literature finds differences in paternal versus maternal
love-related behaviors influencing different aspects of the gender role
development of both boys and girls (Biller & Borstelmann, 1967; Bronson, 1959; Distler, 1965; Kelly & Worell, 1976; Mussen, 1961; Mussen & Distler, 1959; Orlofsky, 1979; Payne & Mussen, 1956).
The work of Jordan, Radin, and
Epstein (1975) serves to illustrate the second type of study in this
category, that is, studies concluding that a single pattern of paternal
love-related behavior is associated with different outcomes for sons versus
daughters. These authors found in a sample of 180 European American children 4
years of age that paternal nurturance was positively associated with
middle-class sons' performance on the Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale (Terman & Merrill, 1960). Paternal
nurturance was unrelated, however, to daughters' performance on the scale.
Finally, a second study is also broadly illustrative of this second type
of research. Here Booth and Amato
(1994) found in a longitudinal study of 419 parents and their adult children
that a poor relationship between spouses while children were living at home was
associated 12 years later with somewhat different outcomes for sons and
daughters. Specifically, adult sons felt somewhat less close to both parents
than did sons whose parents had had a good marital relationship. Daughters, on
the other hand, felt much less close to their fathers but only slightly less
close to their mothers. Thus, the authors concluded, the father–daughter tie
tends to be especially vulnerable in the context of serious marital problems
between parents, whereas the mother–daughter tie tends to be especially
resilient.
Discussion
The body of work reviewed in this article shows that paternal
acceptance–rejection (father love) is heavily implicated not only in children's
and adults' psychological well-being and health but also in an array of
psychological and behavioral problems. Moreover, this body of work suggests that
father love may affect offspring development at all ages from infancy through at
least young adulthood. More specifically, evidence discussed here shows that
father love is often associated as robustly as mother love with a variety of
outcomes. For example, both father and mother love–withdrawal (parental
rejection) have been significantly implicated in offsprings' personality and
psychological adjustment problems, including issues of negative self-concept,
negative self-esteem, emotional instability, anxiety, social and emotional
withdrawal, and aggression; conduct problems, including externalizing behaviors
and delinquency; drug and alcohol abuse; cognitive and academic difficulties;
and forms of mental disorder such as depression, depressed affect, and
borderline personality disorder.
On the other hand, both paternal and maternal love (parental acceptance)
have been shown to be effective buffers against many of these problems, as well
as being associated with a sense of happiness and well-being in adolescence and
adulthood, physical and psychological health, social competence, academic
achievement, and the internalization of parental values as one's own values.
Even though mother love is associated with all of these outcomes, evidence
reviewed in this article suggests that father love is even more strongly
associated with many. Moreover, multiple regression and SEM analyses conclude
that the influence of mother love sometimes disappears altogether, leaving
father love as the sole significant predictor of such outcomes as personality
and psychological adjustment problems, conduct and delinquency problems, and
substance abuse.
Several problems and limitations characterize this body of research.
First, most of the work appears to deal with middle-class European American
parents. Research on the influence of father love among African Americans,
Hispanic Americans, Asian Americans, and Native Americans is relatively
uncommon, and it is equally rare in cross-cultural studies. As a result, it
remains for future research to determine the extent to which results reported
here can be generalized beyond middle-class European American families. Second,
many of the studies reviewed here relied on a single source (e.g., youths' or
parents' reports) to provide information about both parental
acceptance–rejection and the relevant outcome variable(s). As noted by Campbell and Fiske (1959); Marsiglio, Amato, Day, and Lamb (2000);
and others, however, shared-method variance in studies such as these might
artificially inflate the correlation between variables, resulting in an
overestimate of the true effect size.
Third, even though it seems unmistakably clear that father love makes an
important contribution to offsprings' development and psychological functioning,
it is not at all clear why paternal acceptance–rejection is sometimes more
strongly associated with specific child outcomes than is maternal
acceptance–rejection. And it is unclear why patterns of paternal versus maternal
parenting are sometimes associated with different outcomes for sons, daughters,
or both children. Part of the reason for these differences no doubt lies in the
fact that fathers and mothers often interact with their children in somewhat
different ways (Fagot, 1995).
Fathers generally interact with children less frequently than do mothers, for
example, and they generally tend to be less involved in caregiving (Fagot, 1995; K. N. Harris et al., 1998). Moreover,
when they do interact with their children, fathers often initiate different
types of behaviors from those of mothers. For example, fathers tend to engage in
more physical, rough-and-tumble, and idiosyncratic play than do mothers (Collins & Russell, 1991; Forehand & Nousiainen, 1993; Parke, 1996). In addition, fathers are
more likely than mothers to encourage children's competitiveness and
independence and to encourage their children to take risks (Cabrera et al., 2000). Nothing in these
behaviors per se, however, appears to explain why father love sometimes has
different effects from mother love.
Possibly, evolutionary and behavior-genetic influences should be
considered in attempts to explain some of these effects (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington,
& Bornstein, 2000, 2001; J. R. Harris, 1998; Rowe, 1994, 2001). Some of these
differences, however, are probably related to the fact that fathers are often
perceived to have more power and authority within the family than do mothers (Radin, 1981). It remains for future
research to inquire directly about the relevance of these mechanisms. Until then
we can know that father love is often as influential as mother love—and
sometimes more so—but we cannot know for sure why this is true.
Proper recognition of the influence of fathers and father love should
have several salutary effects. First, widespread recognition of fathers'
influence may help motivate many men to become more involved in nurturing child
care. Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, and
Melby (1990) supported this point when they found that "fathers are more
likely to engage in nurturing activities when they believe such behaviors will
make an important difference in the life of their child" (p. 387). Of course,
the degree to which men are likely to become involved in nurturing paternal
behavior is at least partly a function of their mates' internalized cultural
beliefs about the maternal role (Parke,
1995). That is, because of the enormous cultural emphasis placed on the role
of motherhood in America, some women are ambivalent about encouraging fathers to
become heavily involved as nurturing parents (Allen & Hawkins, 1999; Biller, 1993; Cowan & Bronstein, 1988; Doherty et al., 1998; Marsiglio et al., 2000). And of course,
as already mentioned, some men have difficulty redefining masculinity to include
nurturing paternal behavior. Nonetheless, the evidence seems clear that mothers
are more effective parents when fathers are both supportive partners and
nurturing parents. And children are major beneficiaries when both parents are
warm and loving.
A second salutary effect of widespread recognition of the influence of
fathers and their love should be to help reduce the incidence of "mother
blaming" common in the clinical field. That is, according to Phares (1997) and others (Caplan, 1986, 1989; Caplan & Hall-McCorquodale, 1985;
Phares & Compas, 1992), the
great emphasis on mothers and mothering in clinical research and
practice—without examining the influence of fathers and fathering—has led to a
tendency toward blaming mothers for children's maladjustment and
psychopathology.
The emphasis in this review on fathers should not be construed to suggest
that mother love is generally less important than father love. Indeed, some
studies discussing the effects of father love relative to mother love conclude
that mother love has greater influence than father love for specific
developmental outcomes. For example, in their meta-analysis of 15 studies
dealing with the relation between gender of parent and youths' externalizing
behaviors, Rothbaum and Weisz (1994)
found that the correlation between caregiving and externalizing was larger—but
apparently not statistically so—for mothers than for fathers in 80% of the
studies reviewed. According to a related meta-analysis of the relation between
caregiving and conduct problems–delinquency by Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber (1986),
however, 64% of the 75 studies reviewed reported that fathers' love-related
behaviors had a stronger association with these child outcomes than did mothers'
behavior.
Evidence such as this punctuates the need to include both fathers (and
other significant males, when appropriate) and mothers in future research and
then to analyze the data for possible father and mother effects separately (K. N. Harris et al., 1998). It is only
by separating data in this way that behavioral scientists can discern when and
under what conditions paternal and maternal factors have similar or different
effects on specific child outcomes (Cole
& McPherson, 1993; Kim &
Rohner, 2001; Phares, 1996; Phares & Compas, 1992). This call
for separate measurement and analyses of maternal and paternal influences is
contrary to the argument made by some (Kurdek & Fine, 1994; Kurdek & Sinclair, 1988; Schwartz, Barton-Henry, & Pruzinsky,
1985) that behavioral scientists should combine mothers' behavior and
fathers' behavior into a single, composite index.
Beyond this, evidence cited in this article makes even more compelling
the call by Silverstein and Phares
(1996) and others (e.g., Jessor,
1993; Kerr & Bowen, 1988; Parke, 1995; Phares, 1996) to "transform the
dominant theoretical paradigm in the social sciences from a dyadic [i.e.,
mother–child] to a triadic [i.e., father–mother–child], or larger systems model"
(Silverstein & Phares, 1996, p.
48). This transformation would include a paradigm shift in guidelines for
conducting research as well as in graduate training and clinical programs to
automatically include fathers as well as mothers in all parenting matters. In
addition, this call recognizes the need to explore social policy implications of
research showing the powerful influence of father love as well as mother love.
Finally, we should note that many questions remain unresolved and even
unexplored in the research literature assessing the relative contributions of
mothers' and fathers' love and love withdrawal. For example, little is known
about the question of whether children might be differentially affected by
mother love in comparison with father love at different ages or at different
developmental stages. Similarly, it is unclear whether the magnitude of the
effect of mother love or father love varies across offsprings' life span. And,
as already mentioned, little is yet known about why the influence of father love
is sometimes greater than the influence of mother love. Answers to these and
many other such questions await future research.
References
Allen, S. M., & Hawkins, A. J. (1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Mother
beliefs and behaviors that inhibit greater father involvement in family work.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 199–212. [Context Link]
Amato, P. R. (1994). Father-child relations, mother-child relations and
offspring psychological well-being in adulthood. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 56, 1031–1042. [Context Link]
Amato, P. R. (1998). More than money? Men's contributions to their
children's lives. In A. Booth & A. Crouter (Eds.), Men in families (pp.
241–278). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Context
Link]
Amato, P. R., & Gilbreth, P. R. (1999). Nonresident fathers and
children's well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61,
557–574. [Context Link]
Amato, P. R., & Rivera, F. (1999). Paternal involvement and
children's behavior problems. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 375–384.
[Context Link]
Andry, R. G. (1962). Paternal and maternal roles and delinquency. In M.
Ainsworth (Ed.), Deprivation of maternal care (WHO Public Papers, Vol. 14, pp.
31–41). Geneva: World Health Organization. [Context Link]
Arrindell, W., Emmelkamp, P., Monsma, A., & Brilman, E. (1983). The
role of perceived parental rearing practices in the etiology of phobic
disorders: A controlled study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 183–187. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Atkinson, M. P., & Blackwelder, S. P. (1993). Fathering in the 20th
century. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 975–986. [Context Link]
Barber, B., & Thomas, D. (1986). Dimensions of fathers' and mothers'
supportive behavior: A case for physical affection. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 48, 783–794. [Context Link]
Barnes, G. (1984). Adolescent alcohol abuse and other problem behaviors:
Their relationships and common parental influences. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 13, 329–348. [Context
Link]
Barnett, R. C., Marshall, N. L., & Pleck, J. H. (1992). Adult
son-parent relationships and the associations with son's psychological distress.
Journal of Family Issues, 13, 505–525. [Context Link]
Baron, R., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
1173–1182. [Context Link]
Barrera, M., Jr., & Garrison-Jones, C. (1992). Family and peer social
support as specific correlates of adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 1–16. [Context Link]
Bartle, S., Anderson, S., & Sabatelli, R. (1989). A model of
parenting style, adolescent individuation and adolescent self-esteem:
Preliminary findings. Journal of Adolescent Research, 4, 283–298. [Context Link]
Becker, W. C. (1960). The relationship of factors in parental ratings of
self and each other to the behavior of kindergarten children as rated by
mothers, fathers, and teachers. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 507–527.
[Context Link]
Becker, W. C. (1964). Consequences of different kinds of parental
discipline. In M. L. Hoffman & L. W. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child
development research (pp. 169–208). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. [Context Link]
Becker, W. C., Peterson, D. R., Hellmer, L. A., Shoemaker, D. J., &
Quay, H. C. (1959). Factors in parental behavior and personality as related to
problem behavior in children. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 23, 107–118. [Context Link]
Belsky, J. (1998). Paternal influence and children's well-being: Limits
of, and new directions for, understanding. In A. Booth & A. Crouter (Eds.),
Men in families (pp. 279–293). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Context Link]
Benson, L. (1968). Fatherhood: A sociological perspective. New York:
Random House. [Context Link]
Biller, H. B. (1974). Paternal deprivation: Family, school, sexuality,
and society. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. [Context Link]
Biller, H. B. (1981). Father absence, divorce, and personality
development. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development
(pp. 489–552). New York: Wiley. [Context
Link]
Biller, H. B. (1993). Fathers and families: Paternal factors in child
development. Westport, CT: Auburn House. [Context Link]
Biller, H. B., & Borstelmann, L. J. (1967). Masculine development: An
integrative review. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 13, 253–294. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Booth, A., & Amato, P. R. (1994). Parental marital quality, parental
divorce, and relations with parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56,
21–34. [Context Link]
Booth, A., & Crouter, A. C. (1998). Men in families: When do they get
involved? what difference does it make? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Context Link]
Brody, G. H., Moore, K., & Glei, D. (1994). Family processes during
adolescence as predictors of parents-young adult attitude similarity: A six-year
longitudinal analysis. Family Relations, 43, 369–373. [Context Link]
Bronson, W. C. (1959). Dimensions of ego and infantile identification.
Journal of Personality, 27, 532–545. [Context Link]
Bronstein, P., & Cowan, C. P. (Eds.). (1988). Fatherhood today: Men's
changing role in the family. New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Brook, J. E., & Brook, J. S. (1988). A developmental approach
examining social and personal correlates in relation to alcohol use over time.
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 149, 93–110. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Brook, J. S., Whiteman, M., & Gordon, A. S. (1981). The role of the
father in his son's marijuana use. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138, 81–86. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Buri, J. R. (1989). Self-esteem and appraisals of parental behavior.
Journal of Adolescent Research, 4, 33–49. [Context Link]
Buri, J. R., Louiselle, P. A., Misukanis, T. M., & Mueller, R. A.
(1988). Effects of parental authoritarianism and authoritativeness on
self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 271–282. [Context Link]
Buri, J. R., Murphy, P., Richtsmeier, L. M., & Komar, K. K. (1992).
Stability of parental nurturance as a salient predictor of self-esteem.
Psychological Reports, 71, 535–543. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Cabrera, N. J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bradley, R. H., Hofferth, S., &
Lamb, M. E. (2000). Fatherhood in the twenty-first century. Child Development,
71, 127–136. [Context Link]
Campbell, O. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant
validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 2,
31–75. [Context Link]
Campo, A. T., & Rohner, R. P. (1992). Relationships between perceived
parental acceptance-rejection, psychological adjustment, and substance abuse
among young adults. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 429–440. [Context Link]
Caplan, P. J. (1986, October). Take the blame off mother. Psychology
Today, 70–71. [Context Link]
Caplan, P. J. (1989). Don't blame mother: Mending the mother-daughter
relationship. New York: Harper & Row. [Context Link]
Caplan, P. J., & Hall-McCorquodale, I. (1985). The scapegoating of
mothers: A call for change. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55, 610–613. [Context Link]
Carroll, A. D. (1973). Parent acceptance, self-concept, and achievement
of kindergarten children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Auburn University,
Auburn, AL. [Context Link]
Chen, X., Liu, M., & Li, D. (2000). Parental warmth, control, and
indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: A longitudinal
study. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 401–419.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Child Trends. (2000). Indicators of children's well-being. In Trends in
the well-being of America's children & youth [On-line]. Retrieved from http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/00trends/contents.htm#PF[Context Link]
Cole, D., & McPherson, A. E. (1993). Relation of family subsystems to
adolescent depression: Implementing a new family assessment strategy. Journal of
Family Psychology, 7, 119–133.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., &
Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature
and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218–232.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., &
Bornstein, M. H. (2001). Toward nature with nurture. American Psychologist, 56,
171–172.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Collins, W. A., & Russell, G. (1991). Mother-child and father-child
relationships in middle childhood and adolescence: A developmental analysis.
Developmental Review II, 99-136. [Context
Link]
Cowan, C. P., & Bronstein, P. (1988). Fathers' roles in the family:
Implications for research, intervention, and change. In P. Bronstein & C. P.
Cowan (Eds.), Fatherhood today: Men's changing role in the family (pp. 341–347).
New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Crook, T., Raskin, A., & Eliot, J. (1981). Parent-child relationships
and adult depression. Child Development, 52, 950–957. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
DeKlyen, M., Biernbaum, M., Speltz, M., & Greenberg, M. (1998).
Fathers and preschool behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 34,
264–275.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
DeKlyen, M., Speltz, M., & Greenberg, M. (1998). Fathering and early
onset conduct problems: Positive and negative parenting, father-son attachment,
and the marital context. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 3–21.
[Context Link]
Dekovic, M., & Meeus, W. (1997). Peer relations in adolescence:
Effects of parenting and adolescents' self-concept. Journal of Adolescence, 20,
163–176. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Dickens, C. (1843). A Christmas carol. London: Chapman & Hall. [Context Link]
Dickie, J., Eshleman, A., Merasco, D., Shepard, A., Vander Wilt, M.,
& Johnson, M. (1997). Parent-child relationships and children's images of
God. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 36, 25–43. [Context Link]
Distler, L. S. (1965). Patterns of parental identification: An
examination of three theories. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
California, Berkeley. [Context Link]
Doherty, W. J., Kouneski, E. F., & Erickson, M. F. (1998).
Responsible fathering: An overview and conceptual framework. Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 60, 277–292. [Context
Link]
Dominy, N. L., Johnson, W. B., & Koch, C. (2000). Perception of
parental acceptance in women with binge eating disorder. Journal of Psychology,
134, 23–36. [Context Link]
DuBos, D., Eitel, S., & Felner, R. (1994). Effects of family
environment and parent-child relationships on school adjustment during the
transition to early adolescence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56,
405–414. [Context Link]
Duck, S. (1988). Relating to others. Chicago: Dorsey Press. [Context Link]
Duck, S. (1991). Understanding relationships. New York: Guilford Press.
[Context Link]
Easterbrooks, M., & Goldberg, W. (1984). Toddler development in the
family: Impact of father involvement and parenting characteristics. Child
Development, 55, 740–752. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Eldred, C., Brown, Z., & Mahabir, C. (1974). Heroin addict clients'
description of their families of origin. International Journal of Addictions, 9,
315–320. [Context Link]
Ellner, J. (1973). Recent changes in American child rearing practices:
1950 through 1970. Unpublished manuscript, Center for the Study of Parental
Acceptance and Rejection, University of Connecticut, Storrs. [Context Link]
Emmelkamp, P., & Heeres, H. (1988). Drug addiction and parental
rearing style: A controlled study. International Journal of Addictions, 23,
207–216. [Context Link]
Emmelkamp, P., & Karsdorp, E. (1987). The effects of perceived
parental rearing style on the development of Type A pattern. European Journal of
Personality, 1, 223–230. [Context
Link]
Eron, L. D., Banta, T. J., Walder, L. O., & Laulicht, J. H. (1961).
Comparison of data obtained from mothers and fathers on child-rearing practices
and their relation to child aggression. Child Development, 32, 457–472. [Context Link]
Fagot, B. I. (1995). Parenting boys and girls. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
Handbook of parenting (Vol. 1, pp. 163–183). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Context Link]
Ferholt, J., & Gurwitt, A. (1982). Involving fathers in treatment. In
S. Cath, A. Gurwitt, & J. Munder Ross (Eds.), Father and child (pp.
557-568). Boston: Little, Brown. [Context
Link]
Fine, M., Voydanoff, P., & Donnelly, B. (1993). Relations between
parental control and warmth and child well-being in stepfamilies. Journal of
Family Psychology, 2, 222–232.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Fish, K., & Biller, H. (1973). Perceived childhood paternal
relationships and college females' personal adjustment. Adolescence, 8, 415–420.
[Context Link]
Fitz-Simmons, M. J. (1935). Some parent-child relationships as shown in
clinical case studies (Contributions to Education No. 643). New York: Teachers
College, Columbia University. [Context
Link]
Forehand, R., & Nousiainen, S. (1993). Maternal and paternal
parenting: Critical dimensions in adolescent functioning. Journal of Family
Psychology, 7, 213–221.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Fox, N. A., Kimmerly, N. L., & Schafer, W. D. (1991). Attachment to
mother/attachment to father: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 62, 210–225. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Giveans, D. L., & Robinson, M. K. (1985). Fathers and the
preschool-age child. In S. M. H. Hanson & F. W. Bozett (Eds.), Dimensions of
fatherhood (pp. 115–140). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. [Context Link]
Grant, K., O'Koon, J., Davis, T., Roache, N., Poindexter, L., Armstrong,
M., Minden, J., & McIntosh, J. (2000). Protective factors affecting
low-income urban African American youth exposed to stress. Journal of Early
Adolescence, 20, 388–418. [Context
Link]
Green, E. (1982). Role of the father in female athletic achievement.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 43, 10B [Context Link]
Greenberger, E., & Chen, C. (1996). Perceived family relationships
and depressed mood in early and late adolescence: A comparison of European and
Asian Americans. Developmental Psychology, 32, 707–716.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Griswold, R. L. (1993). Fatherhood in America: A history. New York: Basic
Books. [Context Link]
Hall, E. (1849). A mother's influence. Mother's Assistant, 14, 25–29.
Hanson, S. M., & Bozett, F. W. (Eds.). (1985). Dimensions of
fatherhood. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. [Context Link]
Hanson, S. M., & Bozett, F. W. (Eds.). (1991). Fatherhood and
families in cultural context. New York: Springer. [Context Link]
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumptions: Why children turn out the
way they do. New York: Free Press. [Context Link]
Harris, K. N., Furstenberg, F. F., & Marmer, J. K. (1998). Paternal
involvement with adolescents in intact families: The influence of fathers over
the life course. Demography, 35, 201–216. [Context Link]
Heilbrun, A. B., Jr., Orr, H. K., & Harrell, S. N. (1966). Patterns
of parental child rearing and subsequent vulnerability to cognitive disturbance.
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 30, 51–59. [Context Link]
Hewlett, B. S. (Ed.). (1992). Father-child relations: Cultural and
biosocial contexts. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. [Context Link]
Hull, J. G., Tedlie, J. C., & Zahn, D. A. (1992). Moderator variables
in personality research: The problem of controlling for plausible alternatives.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 115–117. [Context Link]
Huttenen, J. (1992). Father's impact on son's gender role identity.
Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 36, 251–260. [Context Link]
Jacobs, M. A., Spilken, A., & Norman, M. (1972). Perception of faulty
parent-child relationships and illness behavior. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 39, 49–55. [Context
Link]
Jain, A., Belsky, J., & Crnic, K. (1996). Beyond fathering: Types of
dads. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 431–442.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Jessor, R. (1993). Successful adolescent development among youth in
high-risk settings. American Psychologist, 48, 117–126.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Jordan, B., Radin, N., & Epstein, A. (1975). Paternal behavior and
intellectual functioning in preschool boys and girls. Developmental Psychology,
11, 407–408. [Context Link]
Kagan, J. (1978, August). The parental love trap. Psychology Today, 54,
57, 58, 61–91.58 [Context Link]
Kaplan, A. (1964). The conduct of inquiry: Methodology for behavioral
science. Scranton, PA: Chandler. [Context
Link]
Kelly, J. W., & Worell, L. (1976). Parent behavior related to
masculine, feminine, and androgynous sex role orientations. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 843–851. [Context Link]
Kerr, M. E., & Bowen, M. (1988). Family evaluation. New York: Norton.
[Context Link]
Khaleque, A., & Rohner, R. P. (in press). Perceived parental
acceptance-rejection and psychological adjustment: A meta-analysis of
cross-cultural and intracultural studies. Journal of Marriage and the Family. [Context Link]
Kim, K., & Rohner, R. P. (2001). Parental warmth, control, and
involvement in schooling: Predicting academic achievement among Korean American
adolescents. Manuscript submitted for publication. [Context Link]
Komarovsky, M. (1976). Dilemma of masculinity: A study of college youth.
New York: Norton. [Context Link]
Kroupa, S. (1988). Perceived parental acceptance and female juvenile
delinquency. Adolescence, 23, 171–185. [Context Link]
Kurdek, L. A., & Fine, M. A. (1994). Family acceptance and family
control as predictors of adjustment in young adolescents: Linear, curvilinear,
or interactive effects? Child Development, 65, 1137–1146. [Context Link]
Kurdek, L. A., & Sinclair, R. J. (1988). Adjustment of young
adolescents in two-parent nuclear, stepfather, and mother-custody families.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 91–96. [Context Link]
Lamb, M. E. (1975). Fathers: Forgotten contributors to child development.
Human Development, 18, 245–266. [Context
Link]
Lamb, M. E. (Ed.). (1981). The role of the father in child development
(2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. [Context
Link]
Lamb, M. E. (1986). The changing role of fathers. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.),
The father's role: Applied perspectives (pp. 3–28). New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Lamb, M. E. (1997). Fathers and child development: An introductory
overview and guide. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child
development (pp. 1-18). New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Lamb, M. E. (2000). The history of research on father involvement: An
overview. Marriage and Family Review, 29, 23–42. [Context Link]
Lamb, M. E., Pleck, J. H., Charnov, E. L., & Levine, J. A. (1987). A
biosocial perspective on paternal behavior and involvement. In J. B. Lancaster,
J. Altmann, A. S. Rossi, & L. R. Sherrod (Eds.), Parenting across the
lifespan: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 111–142). New York: Aldine de Gruyter. [Context Link]
La Rossa, R. (1997). The modernization of fatherhood: A social and
political history. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Context Link]
La Rossa, R., Jaret, E., Gadgil, M., & Wynn, G. R. (2000). The
changing culture of fatherhood in comic-strip families: A six-decade analysis.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 375–387. [Context Link]
Lee, H. (1960). To kill a mockingbird. New York: J. B. Lippincott. [Context Link]
Lefkowitz, M., & Tesiny, E. (1984). Rejection and depression:
Prospective and contemporaneous analyses. Developmental Psychology, 20, 776–785.
[BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Lidz, R. W., & Lidz, T. (1949). The family environment of the
schizophrenic patient. American Journal of Psychiatry, 106, 332–345. [Context Link]
Lidz, T., Parker, B., & Cornelison, A. (1956). The role of the father
in the family environment of the schizophrenic patient. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 113, 126–132. [Context
Link]
Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1986). Family factors as
correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and delinquency. In M.
Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.), Crime and justice (Vol. 7, pp. 29–149). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press. [Context
Link]
Lupton, D., & Barclay, L. (1997). Constructing Fatherhood: Discourses
and experiences. London: Sage. [Context
Link]
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context
of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.),
Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, 4th ed., pp. 1–101).
New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Mackey, W. C. (1996). The American father: Biocultural and developmental
aspects. New York: Plenum. [Context
Link]
Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R. P., & Lamb, M. E. (2000).
Scholarship on fatherhood in the 1990s and beyond. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 62, 1173–1191. [Context Link]
Matsuda, K., & Ritblatt, S. (1998, November). The impact of family
parenting styles on the separation-individuation process among adult
only-children. Paper presented at the 60th Annual Conference of the National
Council on Family Relations, Milwaukee, WI. [Context Link]
McPherson, S. R. (1974). Parental interactions of various levels. Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 158, 424–431. [Context Link]
Mead, M. (1956). Some theoretical considerations on the problem of
mother-child separation. In D. G. Haring (Ed.), Personal character and cultural
milieu (pp. 637–649). Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.
Millen, L., & Roll, S. (1997). Relationships between sons' feelings
of being understood by their fathers and measures of the sons' psychological
functioning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 130, 19–25. [Context Link]
Monkman, J. A. (1958). The relationship between children's adjustment and
parental acceptance. Dissertation Abstracts, 19, 1117–1118. [Context Link]
Mussen, P. H. (1961). Some antecedents and consequences of masculine
sex-typing in adolescent boys. Psychological Monographs, 75(2, Whole No. 506).
[Context Link]
Mussen, P. H., & Distler, L. (1959). Masculinity, identification, and
father-son relationships. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59,
350–356. [Context Link]
Musser, J., & Fleck, J. (1983). The relationship of paternal
acceptance and control to college females' personality adjustment. Adolescence,
18, 907–916. [Context Link]
Nash, J. (1965). The father in contemporary culture and current
psychological literature. Child Development, 36, 261–291. [Context Link]
Orlofsky, J. L. (1979). Parental antecedents of sex-role orientation in
college men and women. Sex Roles, 5, 495–512. [Context Link]
Paley, B., Conger, R., & Harold, G. (2000). Parents' affect,
adolescent cognitive representations, and adolescent social development. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 62, 761–776. [Context Link]
Parke, R. D. (1985). Foreword. In S. M. H. Hanson & F. W. Bozett
(Eds.), Dimensions of fatherhood (pp. 9-12). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. [Context Link]
Parke, R. D. (1995). Fathers and families. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp.
27-63). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Context
Link]
Parke, R. D. (1996). Fatherhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
[Context Link]
Parsons, T., & Bales, R. F. (Eds.). (1955). Family, socialization,
and interaction process. New York: Free Press. [Context Link]
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial
boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia. [Context
Link]
Payne, D. E., & Mussen, P. H. (1956). Parent child relations and
father identification among adolescent boys. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 52, 358–362. [Context
Link]
Peppin, B. H. (1962). Parental understanding, parental acceptance, and
the self concept of children as a function of academic over and under
achievement. Dissertation Abstracts, 23(11), 4422–4423. [Context Link]
Peterson, D. R., Becker, W. C., Hellmer, L. A., Shoemaker, D. J., &
Quay, H. C. (1959). Parental attitudes and child adjustment. Child Development,
30, 119–130. [Context Link]
Peterson, D. R., Becker, W. C., Shoemaker, D., Luria, Z., & Hellmer,
L. A. (1961). Child behavior problems and parental attitudes. Child Development,
32, 151–162. [Context Link]
Phares, V. (1992). Where's Poppa? The relative lack of attention to the
role of fathers in child and adolescent psychopathology. American Psychologist,
47, 656–666.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Phares, V. (1996). Fathers and Developmental Psychology. New York: Wiley.
[Context Link]
Phares, V. (1997). Psychological adjustment, maladjustment, and
father-child relationships. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child
development (3rd ed., pp. 261-283). New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Phares, V., & Compas, B. E. (1992). The role of fathers in child and
adolescent psychopathology: Make room for Daddy. Psychological Bulletin, 111,
387–412.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Pleck, E. H., & Pleck, J. H. (1997). Fatherhood ideals in the United
States: Historical dimensions. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in
child development (3rd ed., pp. 33-48). New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Pleck, J. H. (1997). Paternal involvement: Level, sources, and
consequences. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development
(3rd ed., pp. 66–103). New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Prendergast, T., & Schaefer, E. (1974). Correlates of drinking and
drunkenness among high school students. Quarterly Journal in the Study of
Alcoholism, 35, 232–242. [Context
Link]
Radin, N. (1981). The role of the father in cognitive/academic and
intellectual development. In M. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child
development (2nd ed., pp. 379–427). New York: Wiley. [Context Link]
Radin, N., & Russell, G. (1983). Increased father participation and
child development outcomes. In M. Lamb & A. Sagi (Eds.), Fatherhood and
family policy (pp. 191-218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Context Link]
Radin, N., & Sagi, A. (1982). Childrearing fathers in intact families
in Israel and the U.S.A. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 111–136. [Context Link]
Radin, N., Williams, E., & Coggins, K. (1993, October). Paternal
involvement in childrearing and the school performance of Native American
children: An exploratory study. Paper presented at the Conference on Race/Ethnic
Families in the U. S., Provo, UT. [Context Link]
Rapoport, R., Rapoport, R. N., Strelitz, Z., & Kew, S. (1977).
Fathers, mothers, and society. New York: Basic Books. [Context Link]
Renk, K., Phares, V., & Epps, J. (1999). The relationship between
parental anger and behavior problems in children and adolescents. Journal of
Family Psychology, 13, 209–227.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Reuter, M., & Biller, H. (1973). Perceived nurturance, availability,
and personality adjustment of college males. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 40, 339–342. [Context
Link]
Richter, J., Richter, G., & Eisemann, E. (1990). Parental rearing
behaviour, family atmosphere, and adult depression: A pilot study with
psychiatric inpatients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 82, 219–222. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (1960). Child acceptance-rejection and modal personality in
three Pacific societies. Unpublished master's thesis, Stanford University,
Stanford, CA. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (1975). They love me, they love me not: A worldwide study
of the effects of parental acceptance and rejection. New Haven, CT: HRAF Press.
[Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (1986). The warmth dimension: Foundations of parental
acceptance-rejection theory. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (1994). Patterns of parenting: The warmth dimension in
worldwide perspective. In W. J. Lonner & R. S. Malpass (Eds.), Readings in
psychology and culture (pp. 113-120). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (1995). [Semantic analysis of students' conceptions of
"fathering" versus "mothering"]. Unpublished raw data. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (1998). Father love and child development: History and
current evidence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 157–161. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (1999). Acceptance and rejection. In D. Levinson, J.
Ponzetti, & P. Jorgensen (Eds.), Encyclopedia of human emotions (Vol. 1, pp.
6–14). New York: Macmillan Reference. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P. (2001). Parental acceptance and rejection bibliography
[On-line]. Retrieved from http://vm.uconn.edu/~rohner/CSPARBL.html[Context Link]
Rohner, R. P., & Britner, P. A. (in press). Worldwide mental health
correlates of parental acceptance-rejection: Review of cross-cultural and
intracultural evidence. Cross-Cultural Research. [Context Link]
Rohner, R., & Brothers, S. (1999). Perceived parental rejection,
psychological maladjustment, and borderline personality disorder. Journal of
Emotional Abuse, 1, 81–95. [Context
Link]
Rohner, R. P., & Nielsen, C. C. (1978). Parental acceptance and
rejection: A review and annotated bibliography of research and theory. New
Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P., & Rohner, E. C. (1981). Parental acceptance-rejection
and parental control: Cross-cultural codes. Ethnology, 20, 245–260. [Context Link]
Rohner, R. P., & Rohner, E. C. (1982). Enculturative continuity and
the importance of caregivers: Cross-cultural codes. Behavior Science Research,
17, 91–114. [Context Link]
Rothbaum, F., & Weisz, J. R. (1994). Parental caregiving and child
externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 116, 55–74.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Rowe, D. C. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes, experience and
behavior. New York: Guilford Press. [Context Link]
Rowe, D. C. (2001). The nurture assumption persists. American
Psychologist, 56, 168–169.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Russell, A., & Russell, G. (1996). Positive parenting and boys' and
girls' misbehaviour during a home observation. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 19, 291–307. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Schwartz, J. C., Barton-Henry, M., & Pruzinsky, T. (1985). Assessing
child-rearing behaviors: A comparison of ratings made by mother, father, child,
and sibling on the CRPBI. Child Development, 56, 462–479. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Sears, R. R. (1970). Relation of early socialization experience to
self-concepts and gender role in middle childhood. Child Development, 41,
267–289. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Siantz, M., & Smith, M. (1994). Parental factors correlated with
developmental outcome in the migrant Head Start child. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 9, 481–503. [Context Link]
Silverstein, L. B., & Auerbach, C. F. (1999). Deconstructing the
essential father. American Psychologist, 54, 397–407.
[Fulltext Link] [Context
Link]
Silverstein, L. B., & Phares, V. (1996). Expanding the mother-child
paradigm: An examination of dissertation research, 1986-1994. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 20, 39–53. [Context
Link]
Simons, R. L., Whitbeck, L. B., Conger, R. D., & Melby, J. N. (1990).
Husband and wife differences in determinants of parenting: A social learning and
exchange model of parental behavior. Journal of Marriage and Family, 52,
375–392. [Context Link]
Stagner, R. (1933). The role of parents in the development of emotional
instability. Psychological Bulletin, 30, 696–697. [Context Link]
Stearns, P. N. (1991). Fatherhood in historical perspective: The role of
social change. In F. W. Bozett & S. M. H. Hanson (Eds.), Fatherhood and
family in cultural context (pp. 28–52). New York: Springer. [Context Link]
Stendler, C. B. (1950). Sixty years of child training practices. Journal
of Pediatrics, 36, 122–134. [Context
Link]
Sunley, R. (1955). Early nineteenth-century American literature on child
rearing. In M. Mead & M. Wolfenstein (Eds.), Childhood in contemporary
cultures (pp. 150–167). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Context Link]
Tacon, A., & Caldera, Y. (2001). Attachment and parental correlates
in late adolescent Mexican American women. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral
Sciences, 23, 71–88. [Context Link]
Terman, L. M., & Merrill, M. A. (1960). The Stanford-Binet
Intelligence Scale. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. [Context Link]
Veneziano, R. (1998, February). The influence of paternal warmth and
involvement on offspring behavior: Holocultural evidence. Paper presented at the
meeting of the Society for Cross-Cultural Research, Tampa, FL. [Context Link]
Veneziano, R. (2000). Perceived paternal and maternal warmth and African
American and European American youths' psychological adjustment. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 62, 123–132. [Context Link]
Veneziano, R., & Rohner, R. (1998). Perceived paternal acceptance,
paternal involvement, and youths' psychological adjustment in a rural, biracial
southern community. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 335–343. [Context Link]
Wagner, B., & Philips, D. (1992). Beyond beliefs: Parent and child
behaviors and children's perceived academic competence. Child Development, 63,
1380–1391. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Walters, J., & Stinnett, N. (1971). Parent-child relationships: A
decade review of research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 33, 70–111. [Context Link]
Williams, E., & Radin, N. (1993). Paternal involvement, maternal
employment, and adolescents' academic achievement: An 11-year follow-up.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 63, 306–312. [Context Link]
Williams, S., & Finley, G. (1997). Father contact and perceived
affective quality of fathering in Trinidad. Interamerican Journal of Psychology,
31, 315–319. [Context Link]
Yamasaki, K. (1990). Parental child-rearing attitudes associated with
type A behaviors in children. Psychological Reports, 67, 235–239. [BIOSIS
Previews Link] [Context Link]
Young, M. H., Miller, B. E., Norton, M. C., & Hill, J. E. (1995). The
effect of parental supportive behaviors on life satisfaction of adolescent
offspring. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 813–822. [Context Link]
1In this article, the term parents is defined as whoever the
most important caregivers are of a child. These people are not necessarily
mothers or fathers. But typically they are. [Context Link]
2It would be misleading to imply that all studies of boys'
gender role development dealt with father–son relationships only. The mother–son
relationship was sometimes included. Overall, however, results suggested that a
nurturant father–son relationship is more important to a boy's development of
masculinity than is the mother–son relationship (Biller & Borstelmann, 1967). [Context Link]