Site hosted by Angelfire.com: Build your free website today!
For my books, click here.

(This list excludes keywords and addresses.)


T P Hutchinson: List of papers (from 1993, summaries are given)
---------------------------------------------------------------
 
  1. Hutchinson, T P 1972. Delay at a fixed time traffic signal. 
     II: Numerical comparisons of some theoretical expressions.
     Transportation Science, 6, 286-305.

  2. Hutchinson, T P, and Satterthwaite, S P 1974. Analysis of some
     characteristics of road traffic accidents by use of police reports.
     6th Conference of the Universities Transport Study Group, held at
     the University of Manchester Institute of Science and Technology.

  3. Hutchinson, T P 1974. Urban traffic speeds. II: Relation of the
     parameters of two simpler models to size of city and time of day.
     Transportation Science, 8, 50-57.
  
  4. Hutchinson, T P 1974. The distribution of traffic in towns:
     Further validation of Vaughan's model by correlating its
     parameters with size of city and time of day. Traffic Engineering
     and Control, 15, 770-771.
  
  5. Hutchinson, T P 1974. The communication of transport research. 
     Traffic Engineering and Control, 15, 775.
  
  6. Hutchinson, T P 1974. The control of right-turning vehicles at
     signal-controlled intersections: A comment on a suggestion by
     Al-Salman and Salter. Traffic Engineering and Control, 15, 920-923.

  7. Hutchinson, T P 1975. Factors affecting the times till death of
     pedestrians killed in road accidents. Injury: the British Journal of
     Accident Surgery, 6, 208-212.
  
  8. Hutchinson, T P 1975. Witnesses' estimates of the speeds of traffic
     accidents. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 7, 27-35.
  
  9. Goodwin, P B, Hutchinson, T P, and Wright, C C 1975. The perception
     of vehicle speeds by pedestrians. Zeitschrift fur Verkehrssicherheit,
     21, 13-18.

 10. Jolliffe, J K, and Hutchinson, T P 1975. A behavioural explanation 
     of the association between bus and passenger arrival times at a bus
     stop. Transportation Science, 9, 248-282.

 11. Hutchinson, T P 1975. Factors affecting the severity of injury to 
     adult pedestrians involved in road accidents. 5th International 
     Conference of the International Association for Accident and Traffic
     Medicine, held in London.

 12. Hutchinson, T P, and Jones, I S 1975. The separation of the effects 
     of driver and of vehicle on type of accident. 5th International 
     Conference of the International Association for Accident and Traffic
     Medicine, held in London.
 
 13. Hutchinson, T P 1976. Statistical aspects of injury severity. Part I:
     Comparison of two populations when there are several grades of 
     injury. Transportation Science, 10, 269-284.
 
 14. Hutchinson, T P 1976. Statistical aspects of injury severity. Part II:
     The case of several populations but only three grades of injury. 
     Transportation Science, 10, 285-299.
 
 15. Hutchinson, T P 1976. Combining two-tailed rank-correlation
     statistics. Applied Statistics (Journal of the Royal Statistical
     Society, Series C), 25, 21-25.
 
 16. Goodwin, P B, and Hutchinson, T P 1977. The risk of walking.
     Transportation, 6, 217-230.
 
 17. Hutchinson, T P 1977. Application of Kendall's partial tau to a
     problem in accident analysis. International Journal of 
     Bio-Medical Computing, 8, 277-281.
 
 18. Hutchinson, T P 1977. Intra-accident correlations of driver injury
     and their application to the effect of mass ratio on injury
     severity. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 9, 217-227.
 
 19. Hutchinson, T P 1977. Latent structure models applied to the joint 
     distribution of drivers' injuries in road accidents. Statistica
     Neerlandica, 31, 105-111.
 
 20. Hutchinson, T P 1977. Universities Transport Study Group: 
     A Conference report. Traffic Engineering and Control, 18, 211.
 
 21. Hutchinson, T P, and Mayne, A J 1977. The year-to-year variability 
     in the numbers of road accidents. Traffic Engineering and Control, 
     18, 432-433.
 
 22. Hutchinson, T P 1977. On the relevance of signal detection theory
     to the correction for guessing. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
     2, 50-54.
 
 23. Hutchinson, T P, and Satterthwaite, S P 1977. Mathematical models for
     describing the clustering of sociopathy and hysteria in families:
     A comment on the recent paper by Cloninger et al. British Journal of
     Psychiatry, 130, 294-297.
 
 24. Hutchinson, T P 1977. The method of m rankings when the numbers of
     observations in each cell are not all unity. Computers and Biomedical
     Research, 10, 345-361.
 
 25. Hutchinson, T P 1978. An extension of the signal detection model of
     information retrieval. Journal of Documentation, 34, 51-54.

 26. Satterthwaite, S P, and Hutchinson, T P 1978. A generalisation of
     Gumbel's bivariate logistic distribution. Metrika, 25, 163-170.

 27. Hutchinson, T P 1978. Some statistical methods useful in the
     analysis of road accident data. 1st Course on Crashworthiness in
     Transportation Systems, held at the Ettore Maiorana Centre for 
     Scientific Culture, Erice, Sicily.

 28. Hutchinson, T P, and Harris, R A 1978. Recent trends in traffic 
     injury. Injury: the British Journal of Accident Surgery, 10,
     133-137 (Annotations, 137-138).
 
 29. Grime, G, and Hutchinson, T P 1979. Vehicle mass and driver
     injury. Ergonomics, 22, 93-104.

 30. Hutchinson, T P 1979. On the relative frequencies of collisions
     between vehicles of different masses. Zeitschrift fur 
     Verkehrssicherheit, 29, 61-63.

 31. Hutchinson, T P 1979. A comment on replicated paired comparisons.
     Applied Statistics (Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 
     Series C), 28, 163-169.

 32. Hutchinson, T P 1979. The validity of the chi-squared test when 
     expected frequencies are small: A list of recent research
     references. Communications in Statistics, Part A: Theory and
     Methods, A8, 327-335.

 33. Hutchinson, T P 1979. Four applications of a bivariate Pareto
     distribution. Biometrical Journal (Biometrische Zeitschrift),
     21, 553-563.

 34. Hutchinson, T P 1980. An easy method of calculating approximate
     recurrence risks using a multifactorial model of disease
     transmission. Annals of Human Genetics, 43, 285-293.

 35. Hutchinson, T P 1980. The definition of restraint effectiveness.
     Accident Analysis and Prevention, 12, 81-93.

 36. Hutchinson, T P 1980. Approximating a trivariate normal probability
     that is of special relevance to the multifactorial model of disease
     transmission. Annals of Human Genetics, 44, 107-111.

 37. Hutchinson, T P 1980. An assessment of the usefulness of route advice
     given by British Rail to passengers. Journal of Consumer Studies and
     Home Economics, 4, 135-150.

 38. Hutchinson, T P 1980. Partial knowledge and the theoretical basis
     of linear corrections for guessing. Contemporary Educational
     Psychology, 5, 227-231.

 39. Hutchinson, T P, and Lai, P W 1980. Experience with the use of the
     program CATLIN in analysing categorical data, with examples from
     road accident studies. In M M Barritt and D Wishart (Editors),
     COMPSTAT 1980. Proceedings in Computational Statistics, pp. 515-522.
     Wien: Physica-Verlag.

 40. Zlotnicki, J, Hutchinson, T P, and Kendall, D L 1980. Some problems
     and prospects with commercial vehicle safety, illustrated by case
     reports of accidents involving ergonomic factors. In D J Oborne and
     J A Levis (Editors), Human Factors in Transport Research. Volume 1,
     pp. 151-159. London: Academic Press.

 41. Hutchinson, T P, and Haslegrave, C M 1980. Determination of 
     patterns of human body measurements by use of partial correlations.
     Ergonomics, 23, 475-483.

 42. Hutchinson, T P 1981. A review of some unusual applications of
     signal detection theory. Quality and Quantity, 15, 71-98.

 43. Hutchinson, T P 1981. Causes of death in road crashes: Evidence from
     the routinely-published statistics of several countries. American     
     Association for Automotive Medicine Quarterly/Journal, 3(1), 39-40.

 44. Hutchinson, T P 1981. An assessment of the information given in 
     railway timetable-leaflets. Journal of Consumer Studies and Home 
     Economics, 5, 239-246.

 45. Hutchinson, T P 1981. Compound gamma bivariate distributions. Metrika,
     28, 263-271.

 46. Hutchinson, T P 1981. Disagreements when using ordered categories. 
     Quality and Quantity, 15, 593-596.

 47. Hutchinson, T P, and Adams, V 1981. International statistics of road
     fatalities. Transport Reviews, 1, 393-397.

 48. Hutchinson, T P, and Lai, P W 1981. Statistical aspects of injury 
     severity. Part III: Making allowance for differences in the
     assessment of level of trauma. Transportation Science, 15, 297-305.
 
 49. Hutchinson, T P 1982. Statistical aspects of injury severity. 
     Part IV: Matched data. Transportation Science, 16, 83-105.

 50. Hutchinson, T P 1982. Signal probability and the slope of the ROC:
     A comment on Treisman. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 193-197.

 51. Frary, R B, and Hutchinson, T P 1982. Willingness to answer
     multiple-choice questions as manifested both in genuine and in
     nonsense items. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 42,
     815-821.

 52. Hutchinson, T P 1982. Some theories of performance in multiple-choice
     tests, and their implications for variants of the task. British 
     Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 35, 71-89.

 53. Hutchinson, T P 1982. Disease concordance and sex similarity in 
     twins: Application of a multifactorial model with latent structure.
     Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 36, 155-156.

 54. Grime, G, and Hutchinson, T P 1982. The influence of vehicle weight
     on the risk of injury to drivers. Ninth International Technical
     Conference on Experimental Safety Vehicles, held in Kyoto, 
     pp. 726-741. Washington, D.C.: National Highway Traffic Safety 
     Administration.

 55. Hutchinson, T P 1983. A bivariate normal model for intra-accident
     correlations of driver injury, with application to the effect of mass 
     ratio. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 15, 215-224.

 56. Hutchinson, T P 1983. A note on applications of the competing risks
     model. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 15, 225-226.

 57. Hutchinson, T P, and Roe, M S 1983. Latent structure modelling of
     trip distribution. Compendium of Technical Papers, 53rd Annual
     Meeting of the Institute of Transportation Engineers, held in London,
     pp. 11.22-11.24. Washington, D.C.: Institute of Transportation 
     Engineers.

 58. Hutchinson, T P, Allen, A M, Gornell, A W, and Liew, V K 1983.
     Judging the centres of irregular shapes: How much variability is
     there? Ergonomics, 26, 981-984.

 59. Hutchinson, T P, and Nicholl, J P 1983. Recommendations about 
     collecting bus headway data and estimating expected waiting times. 
     Traffic Engineering and Control, 24, 529-531.

 60. Hutchinson, T P 1984. Medical statistics on road accident injury in
     several countries. In S Yagar (Editor), Transport Risk Assessment,
     pp. 43-76. Waterloo: University of Waterloo Press. An earlier
     version appeared in Covjek i Promet, 9, 17-30 (1983), under the
     title "Deaths and injuries in road accidents: Evidence from the
     mortality and health statistics of several countries" (with 
     "health" erroneously omitted from the printed title).

 61. Hutchinson, T P 1984. Using the bivariate normal distribution to
     construct probability models in the health sciences. Communications
     is Statistics - Theory and Methods, 13, 1723-1733.

 62. Hutchinson, T P 1984. Risk in transport. A symposium report. Traffic
     Engineering and Control, 25, 330-331.

 63. Hutchinson, T P 1984. Cross-national comparison of the routine
     collection of data on the nature and severity of injury in road
     crashes. Presented at the International Workshop on the Methodology
     of Modelling Road Accident and Injury Patterns, organised by the
     International Drivers' Behaviour Research Association, held at the
     University of Sussex.
 
 64. Hutchinson, T P 1984. Nonsense items in multiple-choice tests.
     Presented at the London Conference of the British Psychological
     Society, held at the City University. (ERIC Document ED254537,
     Educational Resources Information Center.)

 65. Hutchinson, T P 1985. Analysing severity data when assessors
     differ in their usage of the categories. The Statistician, 34,
     183-195. (Presented at the Conference on Statistics in Health, 
     organised by the Institute of Statisticians, held at the 
     University of Kent, July 1984.)

 66. Hutchinson, T P 1985. Reliability of motor vehicle fatality statistics:
     An international perspective. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 76,
     413-414.

 67. Hutchinson, T P 1985. Predicting performance in variants of the
     multiple-choice test. Presented at the Fourth European Meeting of the
     Psychometric Society and the Classification Societies, held at the
     University of Cambridge. (ERIC Document ED263177, Educational Resources
     Information Center.)

 68. Hutchinson, T P 1985. Presenting one probability distribution as a
     function of another - some applications. American Journal of
     Mathematical and Management Sciences, 5, 103-123.

 69. Hutchinson, T P 1986. Statistical modelling of injury severity,
     with special reference to driver and front seat passenger in 
     single-vehicle crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 18, 157-167.
     (Presented at the International Workshop on the Methodology of
     Modelling Road Accident and Injury Patterns, organised by the
     International Drivers' Behaviour Research Association, held at the
     University of Sussex, July 1984.)

 70. Hutchinson, T P, Burt, N, Cuzner, A, and Howell, R 1986. Three
     categories of urban bus passengers. Highways and Transportation,
     33(10), 14-16.
 
 71. Hutchinson, T P 1986. Evidence about partial information from an
     answer-until-correct administration of a test of spatial
     reasoning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11, 264-275.
 
 72. Hutchinson, T P, and Rowell, S 1986. Points systems for car
     insurance. Insurance: Mathematics and Economics, 5, 255-259.

 73. Hutchinson, T P, and Alderson, J C 1986. Routine road accident data:
     Publications and their indexing. In J O Asalor, E A Onibere, and 
     G C Ovuworie (Editors), Road Traffic Accidents in Developing
     Countries - Volume 1, pp. 461-488. Lagos: Joja Educational Research
     and Publishers.

 74. Hutchinson, T P, and Barton, D C 1987. A mechanical reasoning test
     with answer-until-correct directions confirms a quantitative
     description of partial information. Research in Science and
     Technological Education, 5, 93-101.

 75. Hutchinson, T P, and Tang, K Y 1987. Letter on "The value of latent
     class analysis in medical diagnosis" by Rindskopf and Rindskopf.
     Statistics in Medicine, 6, 529-530.

 76. Hutchinson, T P 1987. Re "Analytical potential for multiple
     cause-of-death data". American Journal of Epidemiology, 126, 158-159.

 77. Hutchinson, T P 1988. Inter-observer agreement about traffic
     conflicts: A fourth opinion. Traffic Engineering and Control, 29,
     235-237.

 78. Hutchinson, T P 1992. Randomization tests and the generalised
     method of m rankings. Computer Methods and Programs in
     Biomedicine, 37, 163-164.

 79. Hutchinson, T P 1992. In the multifactorial model of disease
     transmission, why is the rank correlation sensitive to choice of
     bivariate distribution? Annals of Human Genetics, 56, 159-161.
 
 80. Hutchinson, T P 1992. Self-indexing. The Indexer, 18, 105-106.

 81. Hutchinson, T P, and Lai, C D 1992. Concepts of stochastic ageing -
     Who cares? Safety and Reliability, 12(3), 7-12.

 82. Hutchinson, T P 1992. Providing information to the traveller by
     public transport. Presented at the 14th Conference of Australian 
     Institutes of Transport Research, held in Sydney.

 83. Hutchinson, T P 1992. Discussion of road accidents in Iran.
     Journal of Engineering, Islamic Republic of Iran, 5, 75-76.

 84. Hutchinson, T P 1992. What do they have in common? Royal
     Statistical Society News and Notes, 19(1), 7.
        This is a brief letter on who are the people who most
     frequently publish comments on other statistical papers; and 
     some statistical features of such publications - the most
     productive fraction p of commentators account for
     approximately a fraction sqrt{p} of papers commented upon.
 
 85. Hutchinson, T P 1993. On macroscopic description of urban 
     traffic speeds. Journal of the Operational Research Society,
     44, 209-210.
        A V Hill and W C Benton (J Opl Res Soc, 43, 1992, 343-351)
     reported on vehicle scheduling problems in which vehicle speeds
     depend on geographical location and time of day. They adopted
     a broad brush approach, and approximated the travel speed from
     i to j, denoted rij, by the average of speeds associated with
     the origin and the destination, (ri + rj)/2. The present
     contribution calls attention to work in which vehicle speed in 
     a city is approximated by a function of the distance from the
     city centre. There is both theoretical and empirical work of
     this kind.

 86. Hutchinson, T P 1993. A guide to bivariate ideas of quantal
     response analysis, as applied in biometrics and econometrics.
     Biometrical Journal, 35, 99-108.
        A review is provided of the several bivariate generalisations
     of quantal response analysis that have appeared in the biometric 
     and econometric literatures. There are three main types:
     (i) where a binary outcome is the result of two stimulants, and
     thus the bivariate distribution of the thresholds for response 
     is relevant; (ii) where three or more alternative outcomes may
     arise from a single stimulant; and (iii) where the response
     itself is bivariate (i.e., two types of response may
     simultaneously be observed).

 86. Hutchinson, T P 1993. The seventh-root formula for a trivariate
     normal probability. The American Statistician, 47, 102-103.
        A simple method of approximating the trivariate normal
     integral in terms of the bivariate marginal probabilities is
     proposed. It is applied to the multifactorial model of disease
     inheritance.

 88. Hutchinson, T P, and Lai, C D 1993. As regards reliability, what
     is in the Encyclopedia of Statistical Sciences? Safety and
     Reliability, 13(1), 13-21.
        The Encyclopedia of Statistical Sciences was published in
     nine volumes plus a supplementary volume over the years 1982 to
     1989. In this paper, a brief guide is given to those of the
     substantial entries concerning statistical reliability which
     may be of relevance to engineers and reliability analysts.

 89. Hutchinson, T P 1993. Kappa muddles together two sources of
     disagreement: Tetrachoric correlation is preferable. Research in
     Nursing and Health, 16, 313-316.
        When assessing agreement between experts, it is important to
     distinguish between disagreements that can and those that cannot 
     be explained by different placing of the boundaries between 
     categories. Cohen's kappa statistic is affected by both types of
     disagreement, tetrachoric correlation only by the second.

 90. Hutchinson, T P 1993. Second attempts at multiple-choice test
     items. Journal of Statistical Computation and Simulation, 47, 
     108-112.
        Sometimes, an examinee answering a multiple-choice item is
     permitted a second attempt when the first response is wrong.
     Presumably, the probability of being correct at the second
     attempt (c2) ought to be predictable from the probability of
     success at the first attempt (c1). But (a) what kind of theory 
     predicts a relatively low c2 for given c1, and (b) what kind of 
     theory predicts a relatively high c2 for given c1? This note 
     conjectures the answers are as follows: (a) some correct
     options are known to be correct, but no distractors are known
     to be distractors, and (b) no correct options are known to be
     correct, but distractors are sometimes recognised as such.
     It is hoped that some reader can supply a neat proof.

 91. Hutchinson, T P 1993. Comments on the multivariate normal integral.
     Journal of Statistical Computation and Simulation, 47, 112-114.
        In the statistical literature, renewed interest in calculating 
     the normal integral in three and more dimensions is evident.
     This notes draws attention to three applications areas where
     multivariate normal probabilities are often required, and which
     have developed their own literatures on this. These areas are
     heredity (the multifactorial model of disease transmission),
     structural failure, and econometric choice theory. A few 
     references are provided, as a way into each of these.

 92. Hutchinson, T P 1993. Discussion of data on the killing by cold of
     fruit fly larvae in mangosteens. The Kasetsart Journal: Natural
     Sciences, 27, 226-229.
        I Burikam and colleagues (The Kasetsart Journal: Natural
     Sciences, 25, 1991, 251-255) have recently quantified the
     relationship between exposure to cold and death of fruit fly
     larvae infesting mangosteens. It is shown here that the results
     are robust with respect to a number of minor changes that could
     have been made in the statistical analysis. But the results are
     very sensitive to the choice of statistical model assumed 
     (probit or logit) - whereas .0032% of observation lie more than
     4 standard deviations above the mean of a normal distribution,
     about 20 times than number (.071%) lie more than 4 standard
     deviations above the mean of a logistic distribution.

 93. Hutchinson, T P 1994. On overconfidence in multiple-choice tests.
     Psychological Record, 44, 253-255.
        D Zakay and J Glicksohn (Psychological Record, 42, 1992, 
     519-524) found a negative relationship between overconfidence and
     performance in a multiple-choice test. The present paper argues
     that, contrary to what they say, the result could be artificial.

 94. Hutchinson, T P 1994. Finding corrigenda to journal articles.
     Australian and New Zealand Journal of Serials Librarianship, 
     4(4), 69-72.
        A description is given of compiling a book that lists 
     corrections, addenda, and comments that were published in
     any of 78 statistics journals over the period 1970-1991. The
     number of articles corrected, added to, or commented on was
     some 3200. A similar but much shorter index listing 276 articles
     in 9 psychometric journals has also been published.
 
 95. Hutchinson, T P 1994. Models for responses to multiple-choice 
     items. Presented at the 12th Australian Statistical Society
     Conference, held at Monash University.
        Some models are proposed for how examinees respond to questions
     in multiple-choice tests. Their predictions about how 
     second-attempt performance is related to first-attempt performance
     are derived. (i) The starting point is the model in which
     knowledge is all-or-nothing: either the correct answer is known
     (and correctly chosen), or the probability of a correct response
     is at the chance level. A feature of this model is that it makes
     no provision for partial information: for example, when a second 
     attempt is permitted at questions initially answered wrongly, the
     model predicts performance to be at the chance level, but this
     contrasts with the empirical finding of above-chance performance
     in this situation. (ii) The next idea to be considered is that the
     examinee may be able to eliminate one or more of the distractors
     as being definitely wrong, and then he or she guesses at random
     from among the remaining options. The prediction of this model for
     the relationship between first- and second-attempt performance is 
     derived. (iii) Putting the above two models together, we can 
     imagine one in which there is some probability of the correct
     answer being recognised as correct, and some probability of a
     distractor being recognised as wrong; and, for simplicity, one
     might take these probabilities to be equal. The prediction for the 
     relationship between first- and second-attempt performance is
     derived. (iv) A few other models are also proposed, including an
     asymmetric model, one that incorporates careless slips, and one
     that incorporates misinformation.
 
 96. Hutchinson, T P 1994. The greatest new development in traffic
     engineering since the traffic signal? Road and Transport Research,
     3(4), 91-94. Reprinted in Traffic Engineering and Control, 36,
     156-157 (1995).
        A triple conflict between traffic streams - for instance, an
     appreciable right-turning flow, as well as heavy straight-through
     flows - is often the key factor leading to limited capacity and
     high delay at a junction, either three-legged T or four-legged
     crossroads. It is, however, possible to design a junction so that
     such a conflict is replaced by three lesser conflicts, each between 
     only a pair of flows. This leads to higher capacity and less delay,
     for a given amount of road space. Such designs have been publicised
     by R Goldblatt, F Mier, and J Friedman (ITE Journal, 64, 1994, 
     35-42). The present contribution draws attention to support given
     by T P Hutchinson's article in Traffic Engineering and Control, 
     15, 1974, 920-923; that support was based on queueing formulae and
     numerical calculations using them.

 97. Hutchinson, T P 1995. Asking sensitive questions in surveys.
     (Classroom Note.) Teaching Statistics, 17, 43.

 98. Hutchinson, T P 1996. Waiting for buses: Size-weighted means. 
     (Classroom Note.) Teaching Statistics, 18, 9.

 99. Hutchinson, T P 1996. Performance on items in conventional
     multiple-choice and in multiple true-false formats. Educational
     Research Quarterly, 19(3), 3-7.
        It is shown how performance on items administered in multiple
     true-false format may be predicted from performance when the items 
     are administered in conventional multiple-choice format. (The theory 
     is essentially that described by T P Hutchinson in Br J Mathl Statl 
     Psychol, 1982, 35, 71-89.)

100. Hutchinson, T P 1996. Nonunique estimates of ability. Journal of
     Statistical Planning and Inference, 55, 262-264.
        For some models, it can happen that datasets arise for which
     the likelihood function has more than one maximum. That is bad
     enough when the parameter just refers to some statistical 
     distribution, but it can also happen with a person parameter
     in item response theory (IRT), and be important in determining 
     the person's educational and career opportunities. (The IRT
     model discussed here is the three-parameter logistic.) The
     problem has practical and philosophical sides to it, as follows. 
     When computing estimates of ability, how is it known that the
     global maximum of likelihood has been found? If the likelihoods 
     at two maxima are very similar, how is one ability or the other
     assigned to the examinee?

101. Hutchinson, T P 1996. On the generalised Friedman test. 
     Computational Statistics and Data Analysis (Statistical 
     Software Newsletter section), 21, 473-476.
        Nonparametric methods for two-way analysis of variance were
     discussed by V W Rahlfs and H Zimmermann (Computational 
     Statistics and Data Analysis, 20, 1995, 101-110). Several points
     stemming from their paper are made in the present contribution.
     The most important are: (a) chi-squared can be a poor 
     approximation to the null distribution of the test statistic,
     and so a randomisation test is desirable; (b) as has been
     argued by J de Kroon and P van der Laan (Statistica Neerlandica,
     37, 1983, 1-14) and others, allowance needs to be made for 
     differing numbers of observations per row.

102. Hutchinson, T P 1997. Mismatch models that permit partial 
     information to be shown. In W J van der Linden and R K Hambleton 
     (Editors), Handbook of Modern Item Response Theory, pp. 481-494. 
     New York: Springer.
        Conventional IRT (item response theory) models make no allowance
     for the examinee having partial information about the question 
     asked. The models are unable to predict what might happen in 
     situations which allow the partial information to be shown.
     Relations between probabilities of correctness in different formats
     of test - for example, with different numbers of options to choose
     from, or permitting a second attempt at items initially answered
     wrongly - do not fall within their scope. The present chapter puts
     forward what it terms mismatch theory, which overcomes this
     limitation. It is in the style of signal detection theory.

103. Hutchinson, T P 1997. Improving rater agreement studies. American
     Journal of Roentgenology, 168, 1382.
        Studies of agreements between raters are important in
     medicine and elsewhere. Several improvements to the reporting of
     such studies are suggested here. Among these are (a) be clear
     about the process of selection of the objects that were judged,
     and (b) do not use Cohen's kappa, but instead calculate
     separate measures of the correlation between raters and of their
     relative bias.

104. Hutchinson, T P 1997. Comments on McQuay et al., Pain, 64 (1995)
     331-335. Pain, 73, 107-108.
        It is sometimes claimed that in studies where the average
     response to treatment is large, the average response to placebo
     also tends to be large. The purpose of this note is to propose
     a possible mechanism by which such a correlation may occur.
     This is that it is a consequence of the treatment and placebo
     groups in a given study being more similar to each other (in
     respect of various situational factors and patient 
     characteristics) than they are to the groups in other studies.
     This idea is applied to data in an article by H McQuay, 
     D Carroll, and A Moore (Pain, 64, 1995, 331-335). The question
     is raised whether, given that the mean placebo response varies
     across studies (for unexplained reasons) from 11 to 29 (the 
     amount that McQuay et al. noted), the treatment-placebo 
     correlation might be as large as 0.87 (which is what McQuay
     et al. found). The answer is that this correlation is larger
     than expected if the proposed mechanism is correct, but by usual 
     criteria is not unreasonably large.

105. Hutchinson, T P 1997. Comment on correlation in skew distributions.
     Journal of General Psychology, 124, 211-215.
        If the marginals of a bivariate normal distribution are 
     transformed (e.g., by exponentiation), the product-moment 
     correlation is reduced. Dunlap et al. (Journal of General
     Psychology, 122, 1995, 365-377) recently discussed this effect in a
     psychological context. The present paper draws attention to related
     work in physical sciences (such as structural mechanics, geology,
     and meteorology).

106. Hutchinson, T P 1997. Radioactivity half-lives considered as data. 
     Journal of Applied Mathematics and Decision Sciences, 1, 67-71.
        Half-lives of radioactive nuclides range over more than 20
     orders of magnitude. It is striking that, nevertheless, statistical 
     laws may be discovered in these numbers: a log-normal distribution 
     provides  a good description. This is shown using the cumulative 
     distribution. The final paragraph of the paper observes that 
     half-lives and disintegration energies are thought of as constants, 
     not as realisations of some random process, and it would therefore 
     be misguided to ask why they should exhibit particular statistical
     features, or to imagine a stochastic mechanism by which they are
     generated.

107. Hutchinson, T P 1997. The binomial and Poisson distributions.
     (Classroom Note.) Teaching Statistics, 19, 68.
        In their concern to distinguish the Poisson distribution
     from the binomial, some books neglect the similarities between 
     these distributions. Both use the ideas of a constant probability
     of an event occurring, and of independence between all the
     opportunities for the event to occur. An example is given,
     in which road traffic passing a point can be modelled using
     either distribution.

108. Myors, B, and Hutchinson, T P 1997. Using simulation to infer
     the structure of vocational interests. In Proceedings of the
     39th Annual Conference of the International Military Testing
     Association, pp. 323-327. Canberra: Defence Publishing Service.
        Vocational assessment methods have been used for
     counselling workers in civilian and military roles for many
     years. This paper proposes new methods of statistical
     inference applicable to the structure of vocational interests.
     Our approach to inference in this area involves
     variables-in-common models constructed in terms of Holland's
     widely adopted theory. We use the models to test varying forms
     of hypothesised underlying structure. One test examines the
     hexagonal arrangement proposed by Holland, another extends this
     in the direction of Gati's ideas. The statistical testing is not
     based upon algebra, but upon simulation of raw data according to 
     the models. The technique is illustrated using a military dataset.

109. Hutchinson, T P 1997. A heteroscedastic bivariate distribution
     arising from a model for rater agreement, and its fitting by
     simulation. Computational Statistics, 12, 497-503.
        The starting point is a dataset showing that two raters, 
     judging proficiency in spoken Russian, appear to disagree more 
     about the relatively expert speakers than about the novices. 
     A bivariate distribution is invented and shown to fit the data 
     better than the bivariate normal distribution does. The chief 
     features of the distribution are that it is a variables-in-common 
     model, true score plus error for each rater, and that the scatter 
     of error is greater when the true score is high than when it is 
     low. The method of fitting the distribution to the data is 
     simulation. Accordingly, an explicit expression for the joint 
     distribution of the two observed scores is not required. The 
     software used has ranking and recoding commands of one line each, 
     so it is easy to ensure the fitted marginal distributions exactly 
     match the data, and it is unnecessary to estimate parameters
     representing the boundaries between the grades of rating.

110. Hutchinson, T P 1997. What effect does an untreated aneurysm
     have on life expectancy? Canadian Journal of Neurological 
     Sciences, 24, 357-358.
        If a patient is found to have a cerebral aneurysm that is
     asymptomatic, should the surgeon operate or leave well alone?
     The present contribution discusses some aspects of a paper by
     R Leblanc and K J Worsley (Can J Neurol Sci, 22, 1995, 30-35).
     A formula is put forward that gives the expected lifetime lost
     when an extra risk (e.g., aneurysm rupture) that is constant
     through time is added to all the existing risks. The formula
     is expressed in terms of the level of this extra risk, along
     with the mean and standard deviation that lifetimes would have
     if that risk were absent.

111. Hutchinson, T P 1998. On a recognition task in which some 
     distractors were half-familiar. International Journal of Aging 
     and Human Development, 46, 21-24.
        Probabilistic models are suggested for the task of recognising
     word-pairs, where the distractors may be pairs of new words, or
     may be a new word paired with a previously-seen word. These
     models are relevant to an experiment recently reported by
     M Isingrini et al. (International Journal of Aging and Human
     Development, 41, 1995, 79-88), and suggest rather different
     conclusions to theirs - namely, that the elderly differ from the
     young in both learning and response-selection characteristics.

112. Hutchinson, T P, and Cairns, D 1998. Difference or ratio:
     A note on two-treatment, four-sequence analysis. Biometrics,
     54, 788-789.
        In a two-treatment four-sequence experiment, patients are 
     randomised to one of the four sequences DD, DP, PD, or PP (D=drug, 
     P=placebo). Elswick and Uthoff (Biometrics, 1989) proposed a 
     "nonparametric" method for analysing such data. This wording may 
     suggest that monotonic but nonlinear transformations have no 
     effect. However, the nonparametric aspect of Elswick and Uthoff's 
     methods only enters after the initial calculations - which 
     consist of contrasting the results in the second period with those 
     in the first - have been performed. Specifically, if the method of 
     contrasting consists of taking ratios of the raw numbers, rather 
     than taking differences, the conclusions for Elswick and Uthoff's 
     example are changed. It is also shown how the software StatXact 3
     may be used in this context. 

113. Lai, C-D, Rayner, J C W, and Hutchinson, T P 1998. Properties
     of the sample correlation of the bivariate lognormal distribution.
     In L Pereira-Mendoza, L S Kea, T W Kee, and W-K Wong (Editors),
     Statistical Education - Expanding the Network: Proceedings of the
     Fifth International Conference on Teaching of Statistics, Volume 1, 
     pp. 309-315. Voorburg: International Statistical Institute.
        Most statistics students know that the sample correlation
     coefficient may be used to estimate the population correlation
     coefficient. If the parent population is bivariate normal, this
     does not cause any trouble. However, if the marginals are
     nonnormal, the estimated value from a sample may be quite
     different from the population value. Our simulations indicate
     that for bivariate lognormal distributions that are highly
     skew, the bias in estimating rho can be very large, and is
     substantially reduced only with millions of observations. This 
     example could serve as an exercise for statistics students to
     realise some of the pitfalls in using the sample correlation to
     estimate the population correlation.

114. Hutchinson, T P 1998. Mean or median for exponential data?
     (Classroom Note.) Teaching Statistics, 20(3), 92-93.
        For a skewed population, it is often considered that the
     median captures the idea of "typical value" better than the mean
     does. But what method should be used to estimate the population
     median? It seems natural to use the sample median for this.
     However, suppose one believes that the parent distribution is
     the exponential. Then it is advantageous to estimate the 
     population median by first calculating the sample mean (and then
     multiplying by 0.693).

115. Chekaluk, E, Hutchinson, T P, and Cairns, D 1998. Repeated
     measures ANOVA for responses developing over time. European
     Journal of Anaesthesiology, 15, 381-382.
        The starting point is a paper by K Abt (European Journal of
     Anaesthesiology, 13, 1996, 427-431). Comment is made on methods
     of comparing the responses at several points in time in two
     groups of patients. The technique of repeated measures analysis
     of variance, and how this is handled in SPSS, is described.

116. Hutchinson, T P, and Myors, B 1998. Comparison of the structures
     of vocational interests of men and women. In Proceedings of the
     1st International Work Psychology Conference. Sheffield:
     University of Sheffield, Institute of Work Psychology
     (ISBN 0 9533504 0 1).
        Anderson et al. (Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50, 1997,
     349-364) reviewed evidence on whether there are gender differences
     in Holland's RIASEC model of vocational interests, and
     re-published seven pairs of RIASEC correlation matrices (one for
     women and one for men, from seven studies). We have re-examined
     these fourteen correlation matrices, and some of our conclusions
     are different from those of Anderson et al. Specifically, we have
     detected asymmetries that are consistent across studies for each
     gender separately, but which are different for women and men. For
     women, correlations involving I or R tend to be large, and
     correlations involving A tend to be small. For men, correlations
     involving S or C tend to be large, and correlations involving A
     tend to be small.

117. Hutchinson, T P 1998. Note on probability distributions for 
     generation time. Journal of Applied Microbiology, 85, 192-193. 
        Ratkowsky et al. (Journal of Applied Bacteriology, 80, 1996,
     131-137) reported on the variation in generation times of a
     Pseudomonas fluorescens culture. They advocated the use of the
     gamma distribution to describe this. However, it is argued in the 
     present contribution that the main message of their paper is that 
     the coefficient of variation (ratio of standard deviation to mean) 
     does not depend upon temperature; what is presented tells us 
     nothing about the shape of the distribution. (The shape in 
     Figure 2 of Ratkowsky et al. is not the shape of the data, but is 
     determined by the coefficient of variation in the data, along with 
     the assumption of the distribution being gamma.)

118. Hutchinson, T P 1998. Two aspects of reliability. Australian 
     Journal of Osteopathy, 9(2), 5.
        This is a short letter drawing readers' attention to the
     distinction between "Do the ratings mean the same thing to the
     two experts making them?" and "Do the experts place the patients
     in the same order?"

119. Hutchinson, T P 1999. Measuring the congruence of worker and
     workplace: A correlational approach. Australian Journal of
     Career Development, 8(1), 18-20.
        Suppose we know the Holland three-letter codes for a worker
     and for a workplace - for example, somebody assessed to be RCI
     may be working in an occupation considered to be CRS. This paper
     considers how to measure the similarity (or congruence) between 
     the codes. Previous methods are briefly reviewed. Then it is shown
     how the familiar idea of correlation may be adapted to the
     problem. Some of the existing methods are shown to be special
     cases of the new approach.

120. Lai, C D, Rayner, J C W, and Hutchinson, T P 1999. Robustness of
     of the sample correlation - The bivariate lognormal case. Journal
     of Applied Mathematics and Decision Sciences, 3, 7-19.
        The sample correlation coefficient R is almost universally
     used to estimate the population coefficient rho. If the pair
     (X,Y) has a bivariate normal distribution, this does not cause
     any trouble. However, if the marginals are nonnormal, particularly
     if they have high skewness and kurtosis, the estimated value from
     a sample may be quite different from the population correlation
     coefficient rho. The bivariate lognormal is chosen for this
     robustness study. Two approaches are used: simulation and algebra.
     Our simulation indicates that for the bivariate lognormal, the
     bias in estimating rho can be very large if rho is nonzero, and it
     can be substantially reduced only after a large number (millions)
     of observations. This phenomenon, though unexpected at first, was
     found to be consistent with our algebraic numerical analysis.

121. Hutchinson, T P 1999. Familial association of disease and the
     structure of trivariate distributions. Annals of Human Genetics,
     63, 539-544.
        In its usual form, the multifactorial model of disease
     transmission assumes that the liabilities to disease have a
     multivariate normal distribution. This paper studies how
     sensitive to this assumption are the quantitative results from
     the model. Accordingly, bounds are established for the
     probability of a child  having a disease, given that both
     parents have it and taking the heritability of the disease to be
     known. Unfortunately, these bounds turn out to be wide. For
     example, a probability that is 0.38 under the trivariate normal
     model may be as low as 0.12 or as high as 0.78 under other
     trivariate models, even if attention is restricted to those of
     variables-in-common form. The broader statistical issue of the
     meaning of trivariate dependence, as distinct from bivariate
     dependence, is also discussed.

122. Hutchinson, T P 2000. Measuring the success of the Holland
     hexagon. Quality and Quantity, 34, 103-110.
        J. L. Holland's approach to personality (and careers that are
     suitable for different personality types) involves scoring people
     on six personality measures and intercorrelating the six scores;
     there is a hypothesis about the relative sizes of the fifteen
     correlations. Here, some statistics are proposed for describing
     how well this hypothesis (and three variants of it) matches an
     observed correlation matrix. These statistics are analogous to a 
     correlation coefficient. A variables-in-common model is given
     that justifies the most parsimonious of the hypotheses considered.

123. Hutchinson, T P 2000. ANOVA with skewed data. Environmetrics,
     11, 121-124.
        Gonzalez and Manly (Environmetrics, 1998, 9, 53-65) discussed
     the merits of randomisation tests, in the context of factorial
     ANOVA when the data are grossly non-normal. The present note
     gives some references to methods that use the ranks of the
     observations (including that of Benard and van Elteren),
     including computer implementations of these methods. Finally,
     the point is made that highly skewed data should not only make
     the analyst think seriously about what transformation is
     appropriate; it should also lead to consideration of what is the
     research question of real concern, and what metric is appropriate
     for answering it.

124. Hutchinson, T P 2000. Assessing the health of plants: Simulation
     helps us understand observer disagreements. Environmetrics,
     11, 305-314.
        A dataset on the health of plants, as judged by two raters,
     appears to show more disagreement about the relatively healthy
     plants than about the less healthy. The bivariate normal
     distribution is shown to be a poor description of the data, and a
     new bivariate distribution is developed that gives a good fit to
     the data. The chief features of the distribution are that it is a
     model with latent variables in common (true score plus error for
     each rater), and that the scatter of error is greater when the
     true score is high than when it is low. As the distribution is
     fitted to the data by simulation, an explicit expression for the
     joint distribution of the two observed scores is not required.
     The software used has ranking and recoding commands of one line
     each, so it is easy to ensure the fitted marginal distributions
     exactly match the data, and it is unnecessary to estimate
     parameters representing the boundaries between the grades of
     rating. The method of obtaining the distribution is very
     flexible. In one illustration of this, the relation between true
     score and the logarithm of the scatter of error is quadratic (not
     linear); in another, the true score and the errors have
     non-normal distributions.

125. Hutchinson, T P 2000. Measuring the agreement of several
     three-letter Holland codes for one person. Journal of Employment
     Counseling, 37, 160-162.
        A method is proposed for measuring the agreement of several
     three-letter Holland codes that have been obtained on one
     individual using different methods. The basis of the method is
     the scores 4, 2, 1, 0 for Holland themes listed first, listed
     second, listed third, and omitted from the three-letter code.
     (This is an extension of a proposal by Miller (Journal of
     Employment Counseling, 1997, 34, 40-43), who was concerned with
     combining several three-letter codes into an average one.)

126. Hutchinson, T P, Chekaluk, E, and Cairns, D 2000. ANOVA applied
     to examination scores. Work Study, 49, 104-106.
        This paper reexamines data published by Johnnie (Work Study,
     1996, 45(6), 22-29) on the performance of two groups of bank
     workers on four examination subjects. This leads to a discussion
     of the application of the analysis of variance in contexts where
     there is one within-person factor (for example, examination
     subject), one between-group factor (for example, urban or rural
     location of person), and persons constitute a random factor
     within the group factor. The analysis of Johnnie's data leads to
     a conclusion that differs from the original - that the two groups
     differ in mean score on one of the examinations.

127. Hutchinson, T P 2000. A syllabus for transport studies. Road and
     Transport Research, 9(2), 62-68. (Brief letter in O.R. Newsletter,
     No 364, April 2001, p. 17.)
        In reviewing the relative merits of bus and rail systems,
     Hensher (Road and Transport Research, 1999, 8(3), 3-21) broadly
     favoured the bus. The present writer supports this specific
     conclusion, but even more important is Hensher's emphasis on
     objective criteria for transport decisions. Suggestions are made
     in the present paper for how a degree course in transport could
     emphasise rational decision-making, and examples of
     technological, operational, political, economic, and
     organisational issues are listed.

128. Hutchinson, T P 2000. Graphing the survivorship of bees. Insectes
     Sociaux, 47, 292-296.
        The statistical distribution of lengths of time (for example,
     of the survival of bees) is often of interest. This paper
     describes graphical methods that are appropriate for such data,
     which typically has a skewed distribution. These methods throw
     light on the question of whether hazard rate is constant. Data
     published by Visscher and Dukas (Insectes Sociaux, 44, 1997, 1-5)
     appears to show increasing hazard rate.

129. Hutchinson, T P 2000. Graphing the death of Escherichia coli.
     International Journal of Food Microbiology, 62, 77-81.
        The graphical presentation of measurements of the progress over
     time of bacterial death or inactivation is discussed.
     Specifically, the Weibull distribution may be a useful
     generalisation of the exponential distribution. Data from
     experiments on the non-thermal death of Escherichia coli reported
     by Shadbolt et al. (International Journal of Food Microbiology,
     49, 1999, 129-138) are reexamined to support this claim.

130. Hutchinson, T P, and Cairns, D 2000. A note on the syntactic
     analysis of Greek translations and compositions. Bulletin of the
     International Organization for Septuagint and Cognate Studies,
     33, 39-46.
        Data from Jobes (BIOSCS, 28, 1995, 19-41) on characteristics
     of Greek syntax in seven texts has been reanalysed, using methods
     of correlation and multidimensional scaling. It is argued that
     (a) the two translations of Esther are very similar to each
     other, and (b) the set of papyri are more similar to the four
     translations (of Esther and Daniel) than to the two original
     compositions (Polybius and Josephus).

131. Hutchinson, T P, Cairns, D, and Chekaluk, E 2001? The construction
     of data to reflect the research objective, and how randomisation
     tests make such data usable. Statistical Papers, to appear.

132. Hutchinson, T P, and Myors, B 2001? Testing Holland's hexagon:
     Explanation and criticism. Quality and Quantity, to appear.

133. Hutchinson, T P 2001? Correlations may surprise. Teaching
     Statistics, to appear.

134. Hutchinson, T P 2001? Discussion of "Flexural fatigue life
     distributions and failure probability of steel fibrous concrete" by
     Singh and Kaushik. ACI (American Concrete Institute) Materials
     Journal, to appear.

135. Hutchinson, T P, and Cairns, D 2001? Discussion of a dataset on the
     effect of context on the speechreading of spoken sentences. Journal
     of the Academy of Rehabilitative Audiology, to appear.

136. Cairns, D, and Hutchinson, T P 2001? Did the gold content of
     Cyzicene electrum coins decline over time? A study using
     elaboration as a statistical strategy. Revue belge de Numismatique
     et de Sigillographie, to appear.

137. Hutchinson, T P 2001? Partial knowledge and answer-until-correct
     tasks in birds and humans. Biometrics, to appear.

138. Hutchinson, T P 2001? Calculation of the expected lifetime lost
     due to an extra risk. Mathematical Population Studies, to appear.