ADHD ezine backcopies | ADHD forum to find support | links to ADHD sites |
|
email us |
In this issue, read an
article by RUSSELL A. BARKLEY who is director of psychology and professor
of psychiatry and neurology at the University of Massachusetts Medical
Center in Worcester.He
suggests that in addition to psychostimulants--and perhaps
antidepressants,for
some children--treatment for ADHD should include training parents and teachers
in specific and more effective methods for managing the behavioral problems
of children with ADHD'
You also will find excellent
books for ADHD children to read, and work through.
by Russell A. Barkley
As I watched five-year-old Keith in the waiting
room of my office, I could see why his parents said he was having such
a tough time in kindergarten. He hopped from chair to chair, swinging his
arms and legs restlessly, and then began to fiddle with the light switches,
turning the lights on and off again to everyone's annoyance--all the while
talking nonstop. When his mother encouraged him to join a group of other
children busy in the playroom, Keith butted into a gamethat was already
in progress and took over, causing the other children to complain of his
bossiness and drift away to other activities. Even when Keith had the toys
to himself, he fidgetedaimlessly with them and seemed unable to entertain
himself quietly. Once I examined him more fully, my initial suspicions
were confirmed: Keith had attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Since the 1940s, psychiatrists have applied various labels to
children who are hyperactive and inordinately inattentive and impulsive.
Such youngsters have been considered to have "minimal brain dysfunction,"
"brain-injured child syndrome," "hyperkinetic reaction of childhood," "hyperactive
child syndrome" and, most recently, "attention-deficit disorder." The frequent
name changes reflect how uncertain researchers have been about the
underlying causes of, and even the precise diagnostic criteria for,
the disorder.
Within the past several years, however, those of us who study ADHD have begun to clarify its symptoms and causes and have found that it may have a genetic underpinning. Today's view of the basis of the condition is strikingly different from that of just a few years ago. We are finding that ADHD is not a disorder of attention per se, as had long been assumed. Rather it arises as a developmental failure in the brain circuitry that underlies inhibition and self-control. This loss of self-control in turn impairs other important brain functions crucial for maintaining attention, including the ability to defer immediate rewards for later, greater gain.
ADHD involves two sets of symptoms: inattention and a combination of hyperactive and impulsive behaviors. Most children are more active, distractible and impulsive than adults. And they are more inconsistent, affected by momentary events and dominated by objects in their immediate environment. The younger the children, the less able they are to be aware of time or to give priority to future events over more immediate wants. Such behaviors are signs of a problem, however, when children display them significantly more than their peers do.
Boys are at least three times as likely as girls
to develop the disorder; indeed, some studies have found that
boys with ADHD outnumber girls with the condition by nine to one, possibly
because boys are genetically more prone to disorders of the nervous system.
The behavior patterns that typify ADHD usually arise between the
ages of three and five. Even so, the age of onset can vary widely: some
children do not develop symptoms until late childhood or even early adolescence.
Why their symptoms are delayed remains unclear.
Huge numbers of people are affected. Many studies estimate that between 2 and 9.5 percent of all school-age children worldwide have ADHD; researchers have identified it in every nation and culture they have studied. What is more, the condition, which was once thought to ease with age, can persist into adulthood. For example, roughly two thirds of 158 children with ADHD my colleagues and I evaluated in the 1970s still had the disorder in their twenties. And many of those who no longer fit the clinical description of ADHD were still having significant adjustment problems at work, in school or in other social settings.
To help children (and adults) with ADHD, psychiatrists and psychologists
must better understand the causes of the disorder. Because researchers
have traditionally viewed ADHD as a problem in the realm of attention,
some have suggested that it stems from an inability of the brain to filter
competing sensory inputs, such as sights and sounds. But recently scientists
led by Joseph A. Sergeant of the University of Amsterdam have shown that
children with ADHD do not have difficulty in that area; instead they
cannot inhibit their impulsive motor responses to such input. Other
researchers have found that children with ADHD are less capable of preparing
motor responses in anticipation of events and are insensitive to feedback
about errors made in those responses. For example, in a commonly
used test of reaction time, children with ADHD are
less able than other children to ready themselves to press one of several
keys when they see
a warning light. They also do not slow
down after making mistakes in such tests in order to improve their
accuracy.
Imaging studies over the past decade have indicated which
brain regions might malfunction in patients with ADHD and thus account
for the symptoms of the condition. That work suggests the involvement
of the prefrontal cortex, part of the cerebellum, and at least two of the
clusters of nerve cells deep in the brain that are collectively known
as the basal ganglia. In a 1996 study F. Xavier Castellanos, Judith L.
Rapoport and their colleagues at the National Institute of Mental
Health found that the right prefrontal cortex and two basal ganglia
called the caudate nucleus and the globus pallidus are significantly smaller
than normal in children with ADHD. Earlier this year Castellanos's group
found that the vermis region of the cerebellum is also smaller in ADHD
children.
The imaging findings make sense because the brain areas that are reduced in size in children with ADHD are the very ones that regulate attention. The right prefrontal cortex, for example, is involved in "editing" one's behavior, resisting distractions and developing an awareness of self and time. The caudate nucleus and the globus pallidus help to switch off automatic responses to allow more careful deliberation by the cortex and to coordinate neurological input among various regions of the cortex. The exact role of the vermis region is unclear, but early studies suggest it may play a role in regulating motivation.
What causes these structures to shrink in the brains of those with ADHD? No one knows, but many studies have suggested that mutations in several genes that are normally very active in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia might play a role. Most researchers now believe that ADHD is a polygenic disorder--that is, that more than one gene contributes to it. Early tips that faulty genetics underlie ADHD came from studies of the relatives of children with the disorder. For instance, the siblings of children with ADHD are between five and seven times more likely to develop the syndrome than children from unaffected families. And the children of a parent who has ADHD have up to a 50 percent chance of experiencing the same difficulties.
The most conclusive evidence that genetics can contribute to ADHD, however, comes from studies of twins. Jacquelyn J. Gillis, then at the University of Colorado, and her colleagues reported in 1992 that the ADHD risk of a child whose identical twin has the disorder is between 11 and 18 times greater than that of a nontwin sibling of a child with ADHD; between 55 and 92 percent of the identical twins of children with ADHD eventually develop the condition.
One of the largest twin studies of ADHD was conducted by Helene Gjone and Jon M. Sundet of the University of Oslo with Jim Stevenson of the University of Southampton in England. It involved 526 identical twins, who inherit exactly the same genes, and 389 fraternal twins, who are no more alike genetically than siblings born years apart. The team found that ADHD has a heritability approaching 80 percent, meaning that up to 80 percent of the differences in attention hyperactivity and impulsivity between people with ADHD and those without the disorder can be explained by genetic factors.
Nongenetic factors that have been linked to ADHD include premature birth,
maternal alcohol and tobacco use, exposure to high levels of lead
in early childhood and brain injuries--especially those that involve
the prefrontal cortex. But even together, these factors can account for
only between 20 and 30 percent of ADHD cases among boys; among girls, they
account for an even smaller percentage. (Contrary to popular belief, neither
dietary factors, such as the amount of
sugar a child consumes, nor poor child-rearing methods have been
consistently shown to contribute to ADHD.)
Which genes are defective? Perhaps those that dictate the way in which the brain dopamine, one of the chemicals known as neurotransmitters that convey messages from one nerve cell, or neuron, to another. Dopamine is secreted by neurons in specific parts of the brain to inhibit or modulate the activity of other neurons, particularly those involved in emotion and movement. The movement disorders of Parkinson's disease, for example, are caused by the death of dopamine-secreting neurons in a region of the brain underneath the basal ganglia called the substantia nigra.
Some impressive studies specifically implicate genes that encode, or serve as the blueprint for dopamine receptors and transporters; these genes are very active in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Dopamine receptors sit on the surface of certain neurons. Dopamine delivers its message to those neurons by binding to the receptors. Dopamine transporters protrude from neurons that secrete the neurotransmitter; they take up unused dopamine so that it can be used again. Mutations in the dopamine receptor gene can render receptors less sensitive to dopamine. Conversely, mutations in the dopamine transporter gene can yield overly effective transporters that scavenge secreted dopamine before it has a chance to bind to dopamine receptors on a neighboring neuron.
In 1995 Edwin H. Cook and his colleagues at the University of
Chicago reported that children with ADHD were more likely than others to
have a particular variation in the dopamine transporter gene DAT1. Similarly,
in 1996 Gerald J. LaHoste of the University of California at Irvine and
his co-workers found that a variant of the dopamine receptor gene D4 is
more common among children with ADHD. But each of these studies involved
40 or 50 children--a relatively small number--so their findings are
now being confirmed in larger studies.
Self-control--or the capacity to inhibit or delay one's initial motor (and perhaps emotional) responses to an event--is a critical foundation for the performance of any task. As most children grow up, they gain the ability to engage in mental activities, known as executive functions, that help them deflect distractions, recall goals and take the steps needed to reach them.
To achieve a goal in work or play, for instance,
people need to be able to remember their aim (use hindsight), prompt
themselves about what they need to do to reach that goal (use forethought),
keep their emotions reined in and motivate themselves. Unless a person
can inhibit interfering thoughts and impulses, none of these functions
can be carried out successfully.
In the early years, the executive functions are performed externally:
children might talk out loud to themselves while remembering a task or
puzzling out a problem. As children mature, they internalize, or
make private, such executive functions, which prevents others from knowing
their thoughts. Children with ADHD, in contrast, seem to lack the restraint
needed to inhibit the public performance of these executive functions.
The executive functions can be grouped into four mental activities.
One is the operation of working memory--holding
information in the mind while working on a task, even if the original stimulus
that provided the information is gone. Such remembering is crucial to timeliness
and goal-directed behavior: it provides the means
for hindsight, forethought, preparation and the ability to imitate the
complex, novel behavior of others--all of which are impaired in people
with
ADHD.
The internalization of self-directed speech is another executive function.
Before the age of six, most children speak out loud to themselves frequently,
reminding themselves how to perform a particular task or trying to cope
with a problem, for example. ("Where did I put that book? Oh, I left it
under the desk.") In elementary school, such private speech evolves into
inaudible muttering; it usually disappears by age 10 [see "Why Children
Talk to Themselves," by Laura E. Berk; Scientific American, November 1994].
Internalized, self-directed speech allows one to
reflect to oneself, to follow rules and instructions, to use self-questioning
as a form of problem solving and to construct "meta-rules," the basis for
understanding the rules for using rules--all quickly and without
tipping one's hand to others. Laura E. Berk and her colleagues at Illinois
State University reported in 1991 that the internalization of self-directed
speech is delayed in
boys with ADHD.
A third executive mental function consists of controlling emotions, motivation and state of arousal. Such control helps individuals achieve goals by enabling them to delay or alter potentially distracting emotional reactions to a particular event and to generate private emotions and motivation. Those who rein in their immediate passions can also behave in more socially acceptable ways.
The final executive function, reconstitution, actually encompasses two separate processes:breaking down observed behaviors and combining the parts into new actions not previously learned from experience. The capacity for reconstitution gives humans a great degree of fluency, flexibility and creativity; it allows individuals to propel themselves toward a goal without having to learn all the needed steps by rote. It permits children as they mature to direct their behavior across increasingly longer intervals by combining behaviors into ever longer chains to attain a goal. Initial studies imply that children with ADHD are less capable of reconstitution than are other children.
I suggest that like self-directed speech, the other three executive
functions become internalizedduring typical neural development in early
childhood. Such privatization is essential for creatingvisual imagery and
verbal thought. As children grow up, they develop the capacity to behave
covertly, to mask some of their behaviors or feelings from others. Perhaps
because of faulty genetics or embryonic development,
children with ADHD have not attained this ability and therefore display
too much public behavior and speech. It is my assertion that the inattention,
hyperactivity and impulsivity of children with ADHD are caused by their
failure to be guided by internal instructions and by their inability
to curb their own inappropriate behaviors.
Such compounds (which, despite their inhibitory effects, are known as psychostimulants) have been found to improve the behavior of between 70 and 90 percent of children with ADHD older than five years. Children with ADHD who take such medication not only are less impulsive,restless and distractible but are also better able to hold important information in mind, to be more productive academically, and to have more internalized speech and better self-control. As a result, they tend to be liked better by other children and to experience less punishment for their actions, which improves their self-image.
My model suggests that in addition to psychostimulants--and perhaps antidepressants,(Click on the link for a natural alternative antidepressant.ed.) for some children--treatment for ADHD should include training parents and teachers in specific and more effective methods for managing the behavioral problems of children with the disorder. Such methods involve making the consequences of a child's actions more frequent and immediate and increasing the external use of prompts and cues about rules and time intervals. Parents and teachers must aid children with ADHD by anticipating events for them, breaking future tasks down into smaller and more immediate steps, and using artificial immediate rewards. All these steps serve to externalize time, rules and consequences as a replacement for the weak internal forms of information, rules and motivation of children with ADHD.
In some instances, the problems of ADHD children may be severe enough to warrant their placement in special education programs. Although such programs are not intended as a cure for the child's difficulties, they typically do provide a smaller, less competitive and more supportive environment in which the child can receive individual instruction. The hope is that once children learn techniques to overcome their deficits in self-control, they will be able to function outside such programs.
There is no cure for ADHD, but much more is now known about effectively
coping with and managing this persistent and troubling developmental
disorder. The day is not far off when genetic testing for ADHD may become
available and more specialized medications may be designed to counter the
specific genetic deficits of the children who suffer from it.
Moss, D. (1989).
Shelly
The Hyperactive Turtle. Rockville, MD: Woodbine Press. . .
Comments: An illustrated story, for young children ages 3-7. |
Nadeau, K., &
Dixon, E. Learning To
Slow Down And Pay Attention. Paperback, 70pp.
Reading level: Ages 9-12 Comments: For ages 6-12, provides age-appropriate information and practical suggestions |
. Making
The Grade: An Adolescent's Struggle With A.D.D.
by Harvey C. Parker (Contributor), Roberta N. Parker, Richard Dimatteo
(Translator)
Reading level: All Ages Comments: A classic, on managing ADHD behaviors for ages ages 9-12. |
.
Putting On The Brakes. New York: Magination
Press.
Comments: General information and coping strategies for ADHD, well written, for ages 8-12. |
Putting
on the Brakes Activity Book
Reading level: Ages 9-12 |
Catch you next month and keep in touch,
we love to hear from you,
Cheers Tricia and Mike Legg
Editors
ADHD ezine
ADHD ezine backcopies | ADHD forum to find support | links to ADHD sites |
|
email us |