Robert Day
Science Education Ph.D . Program
Synopsis
This pilot study aims to evaluate novice biologist's ability to perceive small living specimens in artificial ecosystems. It also uses observations of informant behavior and structured interviews to identify attitudes, educational factors and past experiences that might influence or correlate with the ability to see these specimens. Some specific correlations that will need to be further examined are suggested and possible explanations for observed patterns are explored. The study is part of ongoing research into the role of visual literacy in biology education, that is, the development of students' ability to perceive and interpret biologically useful information in the laboratory and in the real world. This skill is desirable not only in life-science professionals but also in average citizens because it is unlikely that non-science majors will value biodiversity if they are physically incapable of seeing the majority of living species.
The investigation springs from casual observations of certain learning processes in undergraduates, made while I was teaching life-science classes at The Ohio State University and during undergraduate-assisted marine field expeditions. Intrigued by my observations, I resolved to retrospectively examine my own learning process in the fields of cell biology and marine taxonomy. Both areas have one important theme in common; for the novice they involve a large number of encounters with images that are at first visually unfamiliar. In cell biology the first exploration of living things using a microscope exposes the novice observer to shapes, structures, textures and lighting conditions that they do not normally experience in daily life. Similarly for a novice naturalist, some organisms appear to be made up of body parts that are utterly dissimilar to those we might see in familiar vertebrate animals. This is especially true of small invertebrates and organisms in marine or aquatic environments where background textures and lighting conditions are often alien to our terrestrial experience.
When performing demonstrations and discussing lab content with students, I noticed that in both the introductory animal taxonomy and the histology classes, there seemed to be a barrier to learning that was not present in other classes. Specifically, students claimed to be unable to see specimens or structures that I was trying to show them, or they were unable to match a picture of a specimen with its real-world counterpart. Students would draw air bubbles instead of cells, shrimp instead of crabs or algae instead of animals. Students stated that they sometimes felt extremely frustrated because, from their perspective, it sometimes seemed as if they were being asked to observe specimens that simply did not exist. Some complained that the microscope or slides must be faulty because they could not see anything that looked like the line-graphic diagrams in their books. In contrast, experienced instructors usually had no trouble finding specimens, even those they had never seen before, in a matter of seconds.
These observations reveal that students have problems with what I will call "visual literacy", "visual induction" and "visual deduction". When students look at a specimen, novices tend to see it as a two-dimensional isolated whole rather than perceiving it as a three-dimensional collection of parts that they can look for in other specimens. They find it difficult to interpret what they see as either a specific example from which they can infer the appearance of a general set (visual induction), or a new example of something that belongs to a set that they have already seen (visual deduction). These skills are critical if students are to develop a mental picture of what a group of organisms look like in general and if they are to recognize new representatives that may not look exactly like anything they have seen before. Similarly students also have problems developing a realistic idea of just how similar two living things must look in order to be considered the same. The hierarchic nature of taxonomic classification and evolutionary relationships further necessitate the need for biology students to be able to sort, scale and categorize three-dimensional visual information so that they can look for meaningful themes or patterns within related groups of organisms. I call the general ability to seek out, recognize, interpret, categorize and make inferences from visual information "visual literacy".
During my own marine field work, I noticed that animal groups I had recently studied began to appear, as if by magic, at familiar field sites or under my microscope, in much the same way that a reader becomes hypersensitive to the appearance of a new word they have just learned. This indicates that observers in visually complex environments may fail to perceive crucial information if they do not have adequate prior exposure to it.
In this study, informants are asked to observe living things in two small, artificial habitats, part of a collection housed in the Botany and Zoology building. The first system consists of a 1-liter jar filled with water and containing about thirteen macroscopic species arranged in a simple food web. This small, essentially self-sufficient ecosystem has been established for several years and appears quite natural, featuring a tangled community of plants and a stable diversity of small animals. The second habitat was a 10-gallon aquarium filled with empty egg crates and housing stable populations of giant Madagascan cockroaches, a species of mite and a small species of beetle. Informants were also asked to observe a third container filled with pure water as a control to make sure they were not "seeing" any non-existent species.
Informants were asked to point out all the living things they could see. They were not asked to identify them, since this experiment was intended as an analysis of visual perception only, not a test of taxonomic knowledge. However, when accurate identifications were offered, they were noted as a sign of the informant's level of overall background knowledge. Informants were described what they saw and any observational strategies that they employed. All sessions were videotaped for later analysis. After the observational phase of the study was complete, informants were interviewed in order to assess their past educational experiences and attitudes towards living things.
Initial findings indicate that novice biologists do not perceive small living organisms in artificial ecosystems as easily as experienced biologists. In novice biologists, this ability seems to strongly correlate with the extent and type of outdoor activities undertaken and with past educational experiences specifically designed to enhance students' ability to observe living things. Informant indications of empathy for, or positive feelings about living things and the environment also correlate somewhat with visual ability. Detailed and technically accurate knowledge of the mechanics of ecosystems does not seem to correlate with observational ability, indeed students with misconceptions about ecological mechanics do not necessarily seem to be at a disadvantage when it comes to their ability to see living things under the conditions of this study.
Since this study suggests that novice biologists have difficulty visually perceiving small living things, instructors should not assume that students see the same thing that they do during fieldwork or observational exercises. Furthermore, it may be desirable to expose students to fieldwork and other exercises specifically designed to develop the ability to see living things in their natural environment, since these kinds of activities seem to produce a measurable effect.
Summary Handout distributed and used as
basis for discussion at presentation
MSATERS conference, Arps Hall, May 5th
2000
History of the problem:
Show edited videotape exerpts illustrating themes and patterns)
Themes and Patterns:
Refer to exel graphs.
Summary of codes used to categorize observed behaviors and transcripts.
(Possible score range in parentheses)
status: ug = undergraduate,
grd = graduate, novice = non science major,
pro = ecology professional or grad student with
ecological specialty
# seen: The total number of species seen in the two artificial ecosystems. (0-17)
observation: The apparent observational skill of the informant. Considers time spent looking, viewing distance and use of active search and retrieval strategies. (1-5)
ecology: Grasp of mainstream ecological theory, knowledge of science behind environmental issues, absence of misconceptions. (0-5)
outdoors: The extent of exposure to the natural world and outdoor life. Includes camping, hiking watercraft etc. as well as childhood exposure. (0-5)
similar: The informant’s past involvement with similar exercises or training specifically designed to improve field taxonomy and observation skills. (0-5)
taxonomy: Apparent taxonomic
knowledge. Ability to name species. (0-5)
0=cannot identify a single species with either
common or scientific name. 5=names all species, at least to class, uses
some scientific species names.
empathy: Behaviors or statements that show sympathy, respect and fondness of animals. (0=negative, 1=neutral or mixed, 2=positive)
environmental: Positive attitude towards environmental issues. Doesn’t include scientific understanding, only enthusiasm. Must be actively involved in formal activities to score a 5. (1-5)
comfort: The average of the stated and observed comfort level for handling and touching live animals. (0-5) 0 = Cannot even approach bench. 5 = Easily handles cockroaches.
family: The average stated comfort level of other family members with animals. Includes consideration of overall supportiveness of environmentalism of family background. 1=less, 2= same, 3= more (than informant). NOT SHOWN HERE.
"Condensed" codes:
background: The sum of values for ecology, outdoors, similar and taxonomy.
attitude: The sum of values for
empathy, environmental and comfort.